Quantcast
Channel: LearnLikeABoss – Custom Mobile Training Apps Using The Power of Gamification
Viewing all articles
Browse latest Browse all 41

FOSTERING SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING IN AN HONORS CLASSROOM THROUGH UNCONVENTIONAL METHODS AND ASSESSMENT Nancy D. McDonald and Idell McLaughlin presented by learnlikeaboss.com

$
0
0

FOSTERING SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING IN AN HONORS CLASSROOM THROUGH UNCONVENTIONAL METHODS AND ASSESSMENT Nancy D. McDonald and Idell McLaughlin presented by learnlikeaboss.com by LearnLikeABoss - Custom Mobile Training Apps Using The Power of Gamification - World Leader in Gamification!

FOSTERING SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING IN AN HONORS
CLASSROOM THROUGH UNCONVENTIONAL METHODS AND
ASSESSMENT
Nancy D. McDonald and Idell McLaughlin

Brought to you by the scholars at LearnLikeABoss.com

International Journal
of
Self-Directed Learning®
Volume 7, Number 2
Fall 2010
The International Journal of Self-Directed Learning (ISSN 1934-3701) is published
biannually by the International Society for Self-Directed Learning. It is a refereed,
electronic journal founded to disseminate scholarly papers that document research, theory,
or innovative or exemplary practice in self-directed learning. Submission guidelines can
be found at http://www.sdlglobal.com.
SUBSCRIPTION or BACK COPY ORDERS: Contact:
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning
7339 Reserve Creek Drive, Port Saint Lucie, FL 34986
issdl.sdlglobal@gmail.com
© 2010, International Society for Self-Directed Learning. All rights reserved. No portion of
this journal may be reproduced without written consent. Exceptions are limited to copying
as permitted by Sections 107 (“fair use”) and 108 (“libraries and archives”) of the U. S.
Copyright Law. To obtain permission for article duplication, contact the editors at:
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning
7339 Reserve Creek Drive, Port Saint Lucie, FL 34986
issdl.sdlglobal@gmail.com
Cover design by Gabrielle Consulting
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
i
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning
Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
EDITOR
Lucy Madsen Guglielmino, Florida Atlantic University
EDITOR EMERITUS
Huey B. Long, University of Oklahoma (Emeritus)
EDITORIAL BOARD
Naomi Boyer, University of South Florida Polytechnic
Ralph G. Brockett, University of Tennessee
Robert J. Bulik, University of Texas Medical Branch
Rosemary Caffarella,, Cornell University
Philippe Carré, Université Paris Ouest Nanterre La Défense, France
Gary J. Confessore George Washington University (Emeritus)
Richard E. Durr, Online Training Institute
Brian Findley, Palm Beach State College
Paul J. Guglielmino, Florida Atlantic University (Retired)
Joan H. Hanor, California State University San Marcos
Roger Hiemstra, Syracuse University (Emeritus)
Waynne James, University of South Florida
Carol Kasworm, North Carolina State University
William J. Kops, University of Manitoba, Canada
Theresa N. Liddell, School District of Palm Beach County
Patricia A. Maher, University of South Florida
Sharan Merriam, University of Georgia (Emeritus)
Magdalena Mo Ching Mok, The Hong Kong Institute of Education
Albertina Oliveira, University of Coimbra, Portugal
EunMi Park, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine
Janet Piskurich, Stephen S. Foster Medical School, Texas Tech
George Piskurich, ACS, a Xerox Company
Michael K. Ponton, Regent University
Kathleen B. Rager, University of Oklahoma
Thomas G. Reio, Jr., Florida International University
Karen Wilson Scott, Idaho State University
Peter L. Zsiga, Indian River State College
Editorial Associate: Elizabeth G. Swan
Webmaster: Richard E. Durr, Online Training Institute
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
i
i
Preface
This issue touches on several important issues and trends in the field of selfdirected
learning today: the internet and SDL, instrumentation, further exploration of
how leaders use self-directed learning in their leadership roles, and ways of
incorporating self-directed learning into formal instructional settings.
As internet use increases exponentially, options for self-directed learning
become more prevalent, more accessible, and more varied. In the first article in this
issue, Kop and Fournier explore the options and actions of self-directed learners in the
connectivist environment of a Massive Open Online Course (MOOC). They describe
new dimensions of self-directed learning that emerged in their research, which was
conducted under the auspices of the National Research Council of Canada.
Moving from the frontier of internet learning environments to the more
familiar setting of organizations devoted to community service, Phares and
Guglielmino report on an examination of the self-directed learning readiness of
community leaders, describe the types of learning projects that contribute to the
performance of their leadership roles, and document their belief that ongoing selfdirected
learning is essential if they are to fulfill their responsibilities well.
Two articles in this issue address instrumentation. Kirwan, Lounsbury, and
Gibson explore the relationship of self-direction in learning and the Big Five and
narrow personality traits in the Resource Associates Transition to College inventory
(RATTC). Ponton, Carr, Schuette, and Confessore present an analysis of the
usefulness of the Appraisal of Learner Autonomy (ALA), a measure of self-efficacy in
autonomous learning, as a part of the Learner Autonomy Profile (LAP).
Finally, in a practice brief MacDonald and McLaughlin, two gifted professors,
describe creative ways of incorporating self-directed learning into English classes in a
college setting, focusing on creating integrated projects that require independent
thought and effort as alternatives to traditional approaches to instruction and
assessment.
Lucy Madsen Guglielmino, Editor
Huey B. Long, Editor Emeritus
A special note: The IJSDL will now be accepting articles written only using the 6th
edition of the American Psychological Association’s Publication Manual.
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
i
i
i
______________________________________________________________________
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning
Volume 7, Number 2, Spring 2010
CONTENTS
Preface ii
New Dimensions of Self-Directed Learning in an Open-Networked Learning
Environment
Rita Kop and Hélène Fournier 1
The Big Five and Narrow Personality Traits in Relation to Self-Direction in Learning
Jeral R. Kirwan, John W. Lounsbury, and Lucy W. Gibson 21
The Role of Self-Directed Learning in the Work of Community Leaders
Leatrice T. Phares and Lucy M. Guglielmino 35
Research Brief:
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
Michael K. Ponton, Paul B. Carr, Christine T. Schuette, and Gary J.
Confessore 54
Practice Brief:
Fostering Self-Directed Learning in an Honors Classroom Through Unconventional
Methods And Assessment
Nancy D. McDonald and Idell McLaughlin 64
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
NEW DIMENSIONS TO SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING
IN AN OPEN NETWORKED LEARNING ENVIRONMENT
Rita Kop and Hélène Fournier
Abstract
New technologies have changed the educational landscape. It is now possible
for self-directed learners to participate informally in learning events on open online
networks, such as in Massive Open Online Courses. Our research analyzed the agency
and level of autonomy required by learners participating in a course of this nature.
Using Bouchard’s four-dimensional model of learner control, we found that there are
new dimensions to self-directed learning in connectivist learning environments. The
research also brought to light new challenges and opportunities for self-directed
learners who might not be able to call on trusted educators for support in their learning
endeavors, but rely on the aggregation of information and informal communication
and collaboration available through social media to advance their learning.
The proliferation of Information and Communications Technology (ICT) in
recent years has changed the educational landscape. It has added to the complexity of
our lives and aided in the creation of a plethora of new opportunities for learning.
Faculty members are changing their practice and are experimenting with open
educational resources and cloud computing, such as Massive Open Online Courses
(MOOC), acknowledging that informal and self-directed learning now form part of
our everyday existence. The technology, however, raises new challenges and
opportunities for the self-directed learner, who might no longer be able to call on a
trusted educator for support in his or her learning endeavor.
The emerging technologies that are currently shaping the Internet and the Web
provide us with access to information and the ability to work and learn with others in a
creative global collaboration outside the educational structures that have been the
norm for centuries (Downes, 2010; Fournier & Kop, 2010). New structures and
environments are in place where people can learn autonomously, but one might
question if people will be able to do so effectively (Kop & Bouchard, 2011). Two
areas of research are foundational to examining learning in open networked
environments: learner autonomy and connectivism.
Research conducted under the auspices of the National Research Council of Canada
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
Learner Autonomy
Several researchers in the field of self-directed learning see learner autonomy
as an important component of self-directed learning (Ponton, 2005; Bouchard, 2009;
Boucouvalas, 2009). Bouchard (2009) and Boucouvalas (2009) both highlighted the
learning environment, learning context, and the connections people make during their
learning as determining factors in the success of self-directed learning journeys. These
elements are aligned with Bandura’s (2002) ideas on “human agency” (p. 269). He
accentuated three types of agency: personal agency exercised individually, proxy
agency, in which people secure desired outcomes by influencing others to act on their
behalf; and collective agency, in which people act in concert to shape their future in
whatever cultural context they inhabit. Bandura emphasized the importance of all
three agencies and their interrelatedness in the complex world in which we now live.
Tough (1979) and Grow (1991) noted that learners move through different
phases of self-direction, and Bouchard (2009) identified particular factors that
influence autonomous learning strategies. He clustered them in four dimensions, one
dealing with psychological issues, one with pedagogical issues, and two with
environmental issues:
1. The first dimension, which he called the conative one, relates to psychological
issues such as drive, motivation, initiative and confidence. In this dimension
Bouchard also highlighted aspects of context and transitions, how these
influence people’s urges to take up learning, and the social networks that
people are involved in and which act as affective support and resources. He
noted that their past learning experiences might also influence autonomous
learning strategies.
2. The algorithmic dimension relates to pedagogical issues, for instance the
sequencing, pacing and goal setting in learning, the evaluation of progress, and
final evaluation and preparation for validation. These are clearly tasks that in
the past were carried out by the educator; in an autonomous learning
environment, they become issues that learners themselves have to resolve.
Bouchard (2009) also saw two environmental clusters of factors that would
influence learning strategies:
3. The dimension that Bouchard called the semiotics of learning is related to the
delivery model of resources. This model has drastically changed in recent
years and moved from the use of resources such as books and paper to
electronic texts and multimedia, which might be stored in searchable databases
that could be linked through hyperlinks. It could also include contributions in
blogs, wikis, and synchronous and asynchronous communication. Information
is obtained through social networks and learners will need to be able to
evaluate and navigate this new information landscape.
4. The importance of aspects of economy was recognized as a fourth category: the
perceived and actual value of the learning, the choice to learn for personal gain
such as for future employment, and the possible cost of other study options.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
While Bouchard’s dimensions provide an important basis for exploration of learner
autonomy, examining self-directed learning in an open networked learning
environment also requires awareness of the challenges of connectivism.
Connectivist Learning in an Online Environment
A current example of self-directed learning promoted by Downes (2010) and
Siemens (2008) is based on connectivism. They posit that being a member of an
online network, communicating with others and filtering information and ideas that
others provide will lead to knowledge creation and learning advancement.
Connectivism advocates the active engagement of people with resources in
communication with others, rather than the transfer of knowledge from educator to
learner. Moreover, they promote a learning organization whereby there is not a body
of knowledge to be transferred from educator to learner, and where learning does not
take place in a single environment. Instead, knowledge is distributed across the Web
and people’s engagement with it constitutes learning. This model recognizes that the
increasing influence of the Web and the global online connectedness of people will
have implications for people’s learning (Siemens, 2008; Fournier & Kop, 2010). The
role of the educator is predicted to change (Downes, 2010) as learners have the option
to move from a learning environment controlled by the educator and the institution to
an environment where they find their own information and direct their own learning as
they develop ideas and connect with (knowledgeable) others on networks away from
the formal setting.
A connectivist approach and learning environment might pose new challenges
for learners who direct their own learning; but it is likely that such an approach will
also provide new opportunities to enhance their learning experiences. The current
literature related to Web development highlights four challenges and pertinent
developments to connectivist learning:
1. The nature of the network as a place to learn as opposed to a group in an
educational institution and the levels of presence in each has been highlighted
as an important factor in the willingness of participants to actively engage
online (Dron & Anderson, 2007). Power relations in online networks and how
these might influence the information and resources that self-directed learners
will be able to access are other important issues. The structures of the Web are
preventing it from developing into a network where equality is the norm, rather
than the exception (Barabasi, 2003; Boyd, 2010b). In addition, the increasing
influence of commerce on the Web might negatively influence the potential of
the social Web for learning and education (Friesen, 2010, Lanier, 2010)
2. Some literacies have been identified that are critical for learners to be able to
effectively direct their own learning in an open online networked environment.
Apart from reading and writing, these include information and media literacy
and the ability to critically analyze resources and information in order to
understand the new semantics of the Web. Creative abilities and a flexible
mindset in an environment that is characterized by change and complexity
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
5
have also been highlighted as important (Downes, 2009; Partnership 21st
Century Skills, 2009; Sahlberg, 2009).
3. Cloud computing and the emergence of Web2.0 and social media have altered
the dynamics of the Web. Moving away from a linear process of printed text,
they increasingly involve the production of digital artefacts and the storage of
these away from local computing devices, as well as the use of a variety of
communication, collaboration and sharing tools that people find and use on the
Web. These tools have created a new demand on human agency in the form of
creativity, innovation and self-expression (Sahlberg, 2009; Fisher, Giaccardi,
Eden, Sugimoto, & Ye, 2005).
4. The Semantic Web and learning analytics are the latest developments of the
Web and can be used for the visualization of large amounts of data, creating a
need for learners to be able to understand and critically analyze graphs and
figures. The analysis of this “Big Data” can also be used to improve learning in
new ways, and some observers envisage the use of analytics in learning
recommender systems to aid learners in their information aggregation
strategies (Rogers, McEwen, & Pond, 2010; Fournier, Kop, & Sitlia, 2011).
In order to develop empowering learning environments that foster active
learning, designers and developers of such environments first need to understand the
factors that influence people’s attitudes, intentions and behaviours. They must also
understand the prerequisites for people to thrive in such environments in order to
create favourable components and conditions. This paper will investigate whether the
four dimensions that Bouchard (2009) highlighted in his research match the
experiences and perceptions of learners in a Massive Open Online Course that was
held in the autumn of 2010 and if additional dimensions might be justified by
examining their connectivist learning in an online environment.
The Research on Self-Directed Learning in a Massive Open Online Course
(MOOC)
Recognizing the challenges posed by innovations in Web-based learning,
learning technologists have started developing structures to support autonomous
learners in the negotiation of this new and ever-changing learning landscape. Carroll,
Kop, and Woodward (2008) see the creation of a place where people feel comfortable,
trusted, and valued as the crux to engaging learners in an online environment. The
task would be to move towards a space that aggregates content and imagine it as a
community, a place where dialogue happens, where people feel comfortable, and
interactions and content can be accessed and engaged with easily: a place where the
personal meets the social with the specific purpose of the development of ideas and of
learning.
The National Research Council of Canada is in the process of designing and
developing a place that might support autonomous learners online. It is a Personal
Learning Environment (PLE) called Plearn. The development consists of two strands:
The creation of a place, encompassing technological components, where people can
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
6
manage their own learning, and the creation of a pedagogical platform that would
support learners in this endeavor. The research to achieve the design and development
of such a PLE consisted of several strands, but this paper will report only on some
components of the educational research: issues relevant to self-directed learning on a
MOOC.
The Setting
The Massive Open Online Course (MOOC) researched was organized by the
National Research Council of Canada as part of their research in Personal Learning
Environments in cooperation with Athabasca University and the University of Prince
Edwards Island. The subject under scrutiny was Personal Learning Environments,
Networks and Knowledge (PLENK). It was a free course that lasted 10 weeks with a
total of 1641 participants registered. PLENK2010 did not consist of a body of content
and was not conducted in a single place or environment. It was distributed across the
Web.
Two of the facilitators of the course were the founders of connectivism, in
approach to learning that has been earmarked by some as the learning theory for the
21st century. Siemens and Downes (2009) have highlighted on numerous occasions
the importance of human agency and the necessity of active participation in
connectivist learning. They stress the importance of four types of activity for
successful learning: (a) aggregation of information, (b) remixing and reflecting on the
resources and relating them to what people already know, (c) repurposing: creating
something of their own, and (d) sharing their work and activities with others.
The central resource in the course was The Daily, a newsletter that participants
could subscribe to if they wished, which displayed the aggregated resources and
artifacts produced by participants in the course. In addition, the Moodle Learning
Management System with wiki was used to hold discussions and to display course
resources and the schedule for speakers of twice weekly Elluminate sessions.
Throughout the course Twitter and participants’ and facilitators’ blogs developed
around the course subject, and Facebook Groups, Second Life, and other social
network environments were developed by participants.
Learner support was provided by four facilitators in the form of videos,
slideshows, and discussion posts in addition to blog posts, feedback to blogs, and
Moodle discussion posts. Once a week Elluminate was used by facilitators for a
synchronous discussion and chat session on that week’s subject.
Research Methodology
Research in the intricacies of learning taking place on online networks is one
of the axes of the research into the design and development of a PLE. If people are
encouraged to move away from the institution for their learning, it is important to find
out the relevance to the learning experience of the informal (online) networks in which
they find their information and where they might develop. A network in the context of
this paper would be an open online space where people meet, as nodes on networks,
while communicating with others and while using blogs, wikis, audio-visuals, and
other information streams and resources. De Laat (2006) highlighted the complexity of
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
7
researching networked learning and emphasized as key problems the issues of human
agency and the multitude of issues involved, such as the dynamics of the network,
power-relations on the network, and the amount of content generated. Effective
analysis would require a multi-method approach and would involve new ethics and
privacy issues.
New ethics and privacy issues in networked environments. Every
researcher has to consider the ethical implications of the chosen methods of obtaining
the data for a study and the use made of it. Sometimes obtaining data is a matter of
accessing statistics or documents. When human subjects are involved in the research,
careful consideration of the level of informed consent by participants is also required.
Miller and Bell (2002) argued that gaining informed consent is problematic if it is not
clear what the participant is consenting to and where “participation begins and ends”
(p. 53). Several ethical issues were raised in the literature, of which misuse of data and
privacy issues were the most important. Van Wel and Royakkers (2004) and Boyd
(2010a) caution that data could pose a threat to subjects when misused, or used for
different purposes than what it was supplied for. Researchers should at least
anonymise data in order to respect privacy issues (Van Wel & Royakkers, 2004;
Rogers et al., 2010; Boyd, 2010a). It has also been suggested by network researchers
that people should have the choice to opt in or opt out of the use of their data. If
someone is not aware that the data is being collected or how it will be used, he/she has
no real opportunity to consent or withhold consent for its collection and use. This
invisible data gathering is common on the Web (Van Wel & Royakkers, 2004) and
highlights some new decisions related to ethics that researchers will have to make. We
feel that researchers have a responsibility to carefully consider the context of their
research, and also the process that takes place between observing, collecting and
analyzing “Big Data”; data that is left by traces of activities that might not at all be
related to the visible participation of learners.
In this study “Big Data” was captured out on open networks. The research
team set out the boundaries of the research on the consent form that participants were
asked to read at the start of the course. They were informed that data collection would
include learning-related activities in the course environment and also learning
activities that happened outside the course, but where the course tag #PLENK2010
was being used.
Data on PLENK2010 was collected according to these principles: using
quantitative as well as qualitative measures, asking for informed consent, and using
the #PLENK2010 tag to identify course-related data outside the course environment
that learners would consent to include in the research.
Quantitative data collection. Three surveys were carried out near the end of
the course and after it had finished in order to capture and explore learning
experiences during the course: including the End Survey (N = 63); an Active
Producers Survey (N = 32), that was filled out by people after an invitation was posted
in the course blog for people who had produced more than two digital artifacts; and a
Lurkers Survey (N = 74) that was filled out after a similar call for people who had
limited their participation in the course to producing less that 2 digital artifacts and
whose behavior was characterized in a consuming rather than a participating nature.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
8
The Moodle data mining functionality was used to gather participant details, their
level of use and access of resources, information on course activities, and discussions
taking place in the course forums.
Qualitative data collection. In addition, qualitative methods in the form of
virtual ethnography were used. An ethnographer was working on the course, collecting
qualitative data through observation of activities and engagement. She also
interviewed and surveyed a number of participants during the final week and held a
focus group with ‘silent participants’ (lurkers) after the course to gain a deeper
understanding of particular issues related to the active participation of learners. The
researchers were interested in the processes taking place and the perspectives and
understandings of the people in the setting; what Hammersley et al. (2001) describe as
the “details, context, emotion and the webs of social relationships that join persons to
one another” (p. 55). Hine (2005) highlighted that on the Web the technology itself
and the artifacts it produces should be taken into consideration in the online
ethnography, as these are part of the research setting and might influence the human
interactions researched. As vast amounts of discursive data were generated in this
form of networked learning in an open environment, computational tools such as
Nvivo were used for analyses and interpretation of the qualitative research data. It was
fairly easy to capture vast amounts of qualitative data through the aggregation tools
such as the gRSShopper aggregator that was being used to feed into the newsletter
(The Daily).
Data analysis. Learning analytics tools were used as a form of Social Network
Analysis (SNA) to clarify activities and relationships between nodes on the PLENK
network. SNA also provided information on the importance of “connectors” on other
networks, and the most relevant tools to facilitate this. Secondary data analysis was
carried out on the Moodle logs. The gRSShopper aggregator statistics functionality
provided details on course-related use of blogs and micro-blogging tools such as
Twitter. Some analytics and visualization tools, such as the Social Networks Adapting
Pedagogical Practice (SNAPP) tool, were also used to deliver real-time social network
visualizations of Moodle discussion forum activity; while the visualization tool
NetDraw was used to create an ego network for understanding the role of a particular
actor in a discussion.
Because of the volume of data generated by the 1641 participants and
facilitators, quantitative analysis of blog posts and Twitter and Moodle participation
was used, but the analysis of qualitative data was restricted to the Moodle environment
and some blogs that were representative of all the blog posts produced by participants.
Findings
Participants’ Ages and Locations
The professional background of participants on PLENK was mainly related to
education, research, and design and development of learning opportunities and
environments. Participants were teachers, researchers, managers, mentors, engineers,
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
9
facilitators, trainers, and university professors. Figure 1 shows PLENK participants’
age, with a majority of participants in the course over 55 years old.
Figure 2 shows a Google Map, instigated by one of the PLENK participants,
representing participants’ residence. A high number were from the USA, Canada, and
Europe, although participants were from a total of 69 countries.
Participation Levels
When the course started, 846 had registered; participation increased to 1641
by the end of the course, as shown in Figure 3. Twice-weekly meeting sessions were
hosted on Elluminate; once a week with an invited speaker and once as a discussion
session amongst the group and facilitator(s). Actual presence at these synchronous
Figure 1. PLENK participants’ ages.
Figure 2. PLENK participants’ locations.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
0
sessions decreased over the weeks from 97 people in week two, when attendance was
the highest, to 40 in the final week with a similar trend in accessing recordings for the
sessions. Global participation and multiple time zones influenced who could be
present and who accessed the recordings. A high number of blog posts were generated
related to the course (900) and an even higher number of Twitter contributions (3104).
The #PLENK2010 identifier facilitated the easy aggregation of blog posts, social
bookmarking links, such as delicious, and Twitter messages produced by participants,
which highlighted a wide number of resources and links back to participant’s blogs
and discussion forums; thus connecting different areas of the course. Although the
number of course registrations was high, an examination of contributions across weeks
(i.e., Moodle discussions, blogs, Twitter posts marked with #PLENK2010 course tag,
and participation in live Elluminate sessions) suggested that about 40-60 individuals
on average contributed actively to the course on a regular basis by producing blog
posts and discussion posts, while the remaining participants’ visible participation rate
was much lower. Figure 4 shows the number of times people used particular tools, but
does not show how these interactions took place.
Some additional visualizations provided us with some more revealing pictures in
forum discussions and participation while using online tools. We have been
experimenting with several analytics tools, such as the social network analysis tool
SNAPP (Social Network Adapting Pedagogical Practice) used as a bookmarklet to the
browser. The activation of the SNAPP tool resulted in network visualizations and the
data generated was also exported to both VNA (Edgelist format) and GraphML
formats. The creation of the network visualizations clarified the role that an actor
might play in a particular discussion (Figures 5 & 6).
Figure 3. Plenk participation rates. Figure 4. Connections between
participants in a discussion.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
1
Agency and Active Participation
Some people with experience in learning in a MOOC were very involved in the
course. One participant produced a Google Map (see Figure 1) that has received 22307
views and a blog that has been read in 69 countries. The technical tools motivated
several people to produce course-related artifacts. Some examples: one learner
produced a creative concept map (Figure 7). Another used Wordles to ‘skim-read’
papers and develop a visual impression of the content of a paper as shown in Figure 8.
Figure 7. Example of learner concept map
(http://bit.ly/hRBMSR).
Figure 8. Wordle of paper by Drexler on the
networked student (http://bit.ly/g14Gov).
Figure 5. Relationship between topics in a
discussion in week 1.
Figure 6. Learners as nodes on Twitter.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
2
Not all participants contributed in a visibly active way. Many participants
accessed resources, but were not engaged in producing blog posts, videos, or other
digital artifacts. The basis of MOOCs has always been four activities:
1. Actively aggregating.
2. Actively relating these aggregated resources to earlier experiences and
knowledge, what Downes (2009) calls remixing.
3. Actively repurposing; producing a digital artifact with this mix of thoughts.
4. Actively sharing.
It was clear that in this course only a small percentage of participants engaged
in the production of digital artifacts. Between 40 and 60 were active producers; the
other 1580 were not active in this way. This outcome was unexpected to the course
organizers as they saw the production phase as vital to the learning in a networked
environment. As some participants mentioned in the discussion, if nobody is an active
producer, it limits the resources that all participants can use to develop their ideas,
discussion, thinking, inspiration and learning. The research data showed some
interesting reasons why the majority of participants were lurkers, rather than active
producers. As Figure 9 shows, 54.5% of respondents to the lurkers survey indicated
that they have always been self-directed learners and do not think they have to actively
share and reply to discussion forums and blogs to learn. In addition, 50.9% highlighted
that they are tactical lurkers who use particular strategies that are especially useful in
their learning.
Figure 10 indicates that the most important restricting factors to participation
in PLENK were issues outside the course, related to people’s everyday lives, such as
time, job, family, and other commitments, for 80.6% of respondents to the lurkers
survey. Other factors highlighted as important to lurkers were: being a listener and
Figure 9. Explanations of lurking behavior.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
3
reflector, so not being active was the natural thing to do (34.3%) and the perception
that lurking is a legitimate learning strategy (29.9%). Factors related to the chaotic
nature of the course and lack of confidence seemed to be less important, although
novices indicated that it took them time to adjust to the unfamiliar course structure.
For a variety of reasons (e.g. lack of confidence at the start of the course, the
way tools and language were being used, trust and comfort levels, power relations in
the course), lurkers preferred to read and view rather than join into a conversation. An
understanding of the change process itself was also highlighted as important–the
process of transformation and the steps required to achieve it. During the lurker focus
group it was highlighted that novices might need more time for this change process to
occur, especially in relation to building self-confidence and a sense of community in
such a large course. These perceptions were expressed by a participant in the
following blog post:
I’m new to the world of PLNs. I certainly don’t post as much as others but I’m
learning and contributing as I go. Could I be considered a “lurker”? Perhaps,
but I’m getting more and more involved as I go on and as my comfort level
increases. . . . PLNs, despite best intentions can be quite cliquey (sp?) and as a
newcomer, that can be quite intimidating. Will I get more comfortable sharing
and experimenting? You bet! However, I need to do it in an environment
where I feel supported and not judged for my perceived involvement or lack
thereof.
Support by facilitators was highlighted in the literature as one way to make
learners feel more at ease, but this was not confirmed in the end-of-course survey
results. Responses to statements regarding the level of advice and support received
Figure 10. Contributing factors to lurking behavior.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
4
from facilitators and other participants in the course remain ambiguous with regard to
support and feedback mechanisms. This ambiguity is highlighted in the higher
percentages of neutral responses displayed in Figure 11.
A majority of active participants (56.3%) indicated that “Writing and
producing something” was “very important” in their learning and/or active
participation in the course. These same participants also indicated that active
production and interaction with others increased their positive learning outcomes; it
helped them to reflect, involved them in a creative process, and they wanted to give
something back to the group, as shown in Figure 12. However, the others with whom
they interacted did not necessarily have to be facilitators.
Figure 11. Agreement by lurkers with the level of support received during the course.
Figure 12. Why active participation was perceived to be important.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
5
Motivational Issues Relevant to Networked Learning
The end-of-course survey highlighted factors that were important to participant
motivation. What seemed to motivate participants most was finding particularly
striking resources and information, getting involved in an online community, and the
opportunity to learn something new. One participant highlighted, for instance, that
learning alongside self-motivated peers was what motivated her as opposed to
traditional training days where people were forced to be present. Learning how the
new environment might improve their teaching and the learning of others was one of
the motivational factors, while the topic of discussion was another. One participant
highlighted the issues of self-evaluation, self-orientation, and self-regulation as
important in relation to motivation in connectivist learning:
Deciding to build a self-managed PLE must be a strongly (professionally or
personally) motivated choice, and requires a high initial engagement and a
constancy during the time, to be really useful. I put the “strong motivation” in
the top of my list of personal requirements to build and use successfully a
PLE/Ns. That signifies also having clear objectives, before starting a learning
experience: what do I want to achieve? How long I can dedicate to do it? …
Other personal qualities: critical thinking, self-evaluation; self-orientation, selfregulation.
I think the major challenges for people to feel comfortable learning
in PLE/Ns are related to the “self” role, in learning activity.
The relevance of learning to everyday life was highlighted as important by
several learners. One emphasized the importance of having choices at the start of the
learning activity to increase motivation and the need for a negotiation process
regarding content, skills, and process to make courses meaningful and relevant to
everyday life. Affective issues were also highlighted as motivational factors. Some
people found it particularly motivational to be learning about connectivist learning in
the company of the originators of the connectivism theory, while other drew
inspiration from learning in the company of self-motivated persons with a similar
interest. They valued the opportunity to come in contact with, collaborate with, and
meet people who would help to expand their personal network.
Critical Literacies for Learners Operating in an Open-Networked Learning
Environment
Participants found different skills, abilities and competencies important to
learn in a complex learning environment such as in the distributed PLENK2010.
Some emphasized the particular mindset required, while others emphasized during the
lurker focus group that novices might need more time to feel comfortable with this
change process, especially in relation to building self-confidence and a sense of
community in such a large course. One participant commented:
People need to develop . . . a host of new critical literacies in order to learn and
to work effectively with intelligent data, with people, and within the network.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
6
I see the PLE as a way to process data, expand learning capacities of
participants, and grow the network.
Participants highlighted a role for the educator in supporting this development:
for instance, by introducing them to tools and resources and by teaching them how to
critically evaluate information while using these new resources. Participants also
emphasized responsibility for their own learning and their own lives in the new
learning paradigm.
Discussion and Conclusions
The level of activity by participants in the course was particularly interesting.
Although course organizers and promoters of connectivist learning posit that actively
producing digital artifacts is an important stage in the networked learning process,
most participants had a different view and participated in a different way. The large
group of silent participants, “lurkers,” who did not produce artifacts nor participate
extensively in discussions, felt that they were actively engaged in the course through
the other three activities: aggregating information, remixing of it and sharing it with
others. The percentage of lurkers was similar to that of consumers versus producers on
the Web as identified by other researchers and consequently should not be seen as too
low (Nielsen, 2006). Our research showed that people were actively engaged in these
other activities, although the sharing mostly took place outside the PLENK course
structure, in their workplaces or at home and sometimes after the course had finished
because people needed time to think and reflect on the resources, information, and
communication made available during the course. Agency and activity are required in
an autonomous learning environment, but it was clear that learners have their own
ideas on what type of activities would suit them and their lifestyles, which might not
necessarily be the same as those of the course organizers.
Some of the dimensions delineated by Bouchard (2009) clearly influenced the
level of participation and types of activities learners engaged in. The conative factors,
related to psychological factors such as drive, motivation, and confidence, were
important. Participants who had already engaged in MOOCs before this course clearly
participated more in the active production stage than novices, while they also
motivated novices by sharing new tools relevant to educational practice. Novices also
indicated their lack of confidence at participating on a worldwide stage where experts
in the field of PLEs were sharing their research; they highlighted the power-relations
as an inhibitor. On the other hand, these high-profile contributors were mentioned by
others as a motivational factor to participation in the course. Opportunities to exploit
the expertise in the MOOC amongst willing and active participants are therefore worth
exploring in future courses.
Time management, goal setting, and time availability were mentioned as the
most important algorithmic factors influencing people’s participation. Learners found
it hard to pace themselves and were, especially at the start, overwhelmed by the
volume of resources and communication that needed to be managed, shaped, and
organized, even though facilitators told participants that it would be impossible to read
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
7
and view everything that would come their way. People did make decisions about this
at a later stage and devised coping strategies with the help of others.
It seemed that the semiotic dimension as highlighted by Bouchard (2009), the
way in which people would access particular types of information and resources, was
very important as it was different from what participants were used to in the past.
Participants valued the new (to them) and different ways of aggregating information,
by using RSS feeds and (#) tags through social networks and new tools. It was
important for learners to learn about new tools and find out what these could mean for
their own teaching practice. Participants helped each other to find tools that could aid
them in supporting their learning and information aggregation.
The economic factors were also relevant to the course participants. Learners
were intrinsically motivated to participate and placed a high value on the knowledge
they developed on the course subject, Personal Learning Environments, Networks and
Knowledge, and the new tools they could use to enhance their own teaching and work
practice, as well as the extension of their personal networks.
Additional issues played a role in learners’ participation and engagement, the
major ones being the critical literacies required to learn actively in an open networked
learning environment, such as a different mind-set and higher level of critical analysis
of resources than is the case in a more organized classroom environment. People
should clearly not have an aversion to risk and change to benefit most from learning in
a MOOC. This ability to thrive in a changing environment will be influenced by all
four of Bouchard’s factors, and the research showed that there is an inter-relatedness
of Bouchard’s (2009) dimensions.
Based on analysis of the findings, it seems that to bring out the creative
potential in people and to inspire them into the production of digital artifacts,
dimensions of activity, engagement, and learning would have to be heightened and at
their most favorable. Heightening the level of engagement and active participation is
one of the main challenges of learning in an open networked environment and one in
which educators could play a role. Educators and institutions might introduce more
openness in the curriculum by using social media and global participation outside the
boundaries of the institutional classroom to invigorate the learning experience of their
students. Their participation as a critical knowledgeable other on the network could, at
the same time, enhance the thinking process of all involved.
The combination of research methods used, and especially the use of analytics,
added to the understanding of learning in a distributed, open networked environment.
The analytics provided some clarity on the nature of the interactions between course
participants, resources and networks; however, the ethnographic approach, using
comment functions on blogs and questionnaires, was indispensable in arriving at an indepth
understanding of the learning process and the learning experience of
participants. For instance, data regarding the learning experience of passive learners
(lurkers) would have been impossible to obtain without these measures. This paper
presents preliminary research findings and a more in-depth analysis is currently in
progress. We expect that results of these analyses will provide us with indications of
the most favorable conditions for facilitating learning for all participants in an online
networked learning environment.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
8
References
Bandura, A. (2002). Social cognitive theory in cultural context. Applied Psychology:
An International Review, 51(2), 269-290.
Barabasi, A. (2003). Linked: How everything is connected to everything else and what
it means. New York, NY: Penguin Books.
Bouchard, P. (2009). Pedagogy without a teacher: What are the limits? International
Journal of Self-Directed Learning, 6(2), 13-22. Retrieved from
http://www.sdlglobal.com
Boucouvalas, M. (2009). Revisiting the concept of Self in self-directed learning:
Towards a more robust construct for research and practice in a global context.
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning, 6(1), 1-9. Retrieved from
http://www.sdlglobal.com
Boyd, D. (2010a). Privacy and publicity in the context of Big Data. Retrieved from
http://www.danah.org/papers/talks/2010/WWW2010.html
Boyd, D. (2010b). Streams of content, limited attention: The flow of information
through social media. EDUCAUSE Review, 45(5), 26–36.
Carroll, F., Kop, R., & Woodward, C. (2008, November). Sowing the seeds of learner
autonomy: Transforming the VLE into a Third Place through the use of Web
2.0 tools. Paper presented at ECEL-European Conference on e-Learning,
University of Cyprus, Cyprus.
De Laat, M. (2006). Networked learning (Doctoral thesis, Utrecht Universiteit, The
Netherlands). Retrieved from http://www.e-learning.nl/files/dissertatie
maarten.pdf
Dron, J. & Anderson, T. (2007). Collectives, networks, and groups in social software
for e-learning. Paper presented at World Conference on E-Learning in
Corporate, Government, Healthcare, and Higher Education (ELEARN),
Quebec City, Quebec, Canada.
Downes, S. (2009). New tools for personal learning. Procedings of the MEFANET
Conference, Brno, Czech Republic. Retrieved from http://bit.ly/5Xx1L6
Downes, S. (2010, December 5). The role of the educator. Huffingdon Post.
Retrieved from http://huff.to/g7Orh9
Fischer, G., Giaccardi, E., Eden, H., Sugimoto, M., & Ye, Y. (2005). Beyond binary
choices: Integrating individual and social creativity. International Journal of
Human-Computer Studies, 63(4-5), 482-512.
Fournier, H., & Kop, R. (2010). Researching the design and development of a
Personal Learning Environment. Paper presented at the PLE Conference,
Barcelona, Spain. Retrieved from http://bit.ly/c5ehX4
Fournier, H., Kop, R. & Sitlia, H. (2011). The value of learning analytics to networked
learning on a Personal Learning Environment. Paper presented at the 1st
International Conference on Learning analytics and Knowledge, Banff,
Alberta, Canada.
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
1
9
Friesen, N. (2010). Education and the social Web: Connective learning and the
commercial imperative. First Monday, 15(12). Retrieved from
http://bit.ly/heknGm
Grow, G. (1991). Teaching learners to be self-directed. Adult Education Quarterly,
41(3), 125-149. Retrieved from http://bit.ly/goD6Vs
Hammersley, M., Gomm, R., Woods, P., Faulkner, D., Swan, J., Baker, S., Bird, M.,
… Perrott, M. (2001). Research methods in education handbook. Milton
Keynes, UK: Open University Press.
Hine, C. (2005). Internet research and the sociology of cyber-social scientific
knowledge. The Information Society, 21, 239-248.
Kop, R., & Bouchard, P. (2011). The role of adult educators in the age of social media.
In M. Thomas (Ed.), Digital education: Opportunities for social collaboration
(pp. 61-80). New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
Lanier, J. (2010). You are not a gadget: A manifesto. New York, NY: Random House.
Miller, T., & Bell, L. (2002). Consenting to what? Issues of access, gate-keeping and
‘informed’ consent. In M. Mauthner, M. Birch, J. Jessop, & T. Miller (Eds.),
Ethics in qualitative research (pp. 53-69). London, UK: Seven Oakes and New
Delhi: Sage Publications.
Nielsen, J. (2006). Participation inequality: Encouraging more users to contribute.
Alertbox. Retrieved from http://bit.ly/1N1aR5
Partnership for 21st Century Skills. (2009). The MILE Guide: Milestones for the
improving learning & education. Retrieved from http://bit.ly/1FRLVy
Ponton, M. (2005). The relationship between self-efficacy and autonomous learning:
The development of new instrumentation. International Journal of Self-
Directed Learning, 5(2), 50-58. Retrieved from http://www.sdlglobal.com
Rogers, P. C., McEwen, M. R., & Pond, S. (2010). The use of web analytics in the
design and evaluation of distance education. In G. Veletsianos (Ed.), Using
emerging technologies in distance education (pp. 231-248). Alberta, Canada:
Athabasca University Press.
Sahlberg, P. (2009). Creativity and innovation through lifelong learning. Lifelong
Learning in Europe Journal, 14(1), 53-60. Retrieved from http://bit.ly/f4ourS
Siemens, G. (2008). Learning and knowing in networks: Changing roles for educators
and designers. Paper presented at the University of Georgia IT Forum, Athens,
GA. Retrieved from http://bit.ly/170fVM
Siemens, G., & Downes, S. (2008/9). Connectivism and connected knowledge.
Retrieved from http://bit.ly/358Wms
Tough, A. M. (1979). Choosing to learn. Toronto: The Ontario Institute for Studies in
Education (OISE).
Van Wel, L., & Royakkers, L. (2004). Ethical issues in web data mining. Ethics and
Information Technology, 6, 129-140.
Rita Kop (Frederika.Kop@nrc-cnrc.gc.ca) is a researcher at the NRC Canada’s IIT.
Her research focuses on learning in networked environments. Before joining NRC she
was Assistant Professor in Adult Continuing Education in the UK, after a career in
Dimensions to SDL in an Open-Networked Environment
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
0
Dutch elementary education. Her research interests are personalized learning, selfdirected
learning, and learning analytics.
Hélène Fournier (Helene.Fournier@nrc-cnrc.gc.ca) has been a Research Officer at
the National Research Council Canada’s Institute for Information Technology since
2002 and holds a Ph.D. in Educational Psychology from McGill University. Recently,
she has been engaged in researching the design and development of a Personal
Learning Environment.
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
1
SELF-DIRECTION IN LEARNING AND PERSONALITY: THE
BIG FIVE AND NARROW PERSONALITY TRAITS IN
RELATION TO LEARNER SELF-DIRECTION
Jeral R. Kirwan, John W. Lounsbury, and Lucy W. Gibson
Abstract
Based on a sample of 2102 college students, learner self-direction was found to
be significantly related to four of the Big Five traits: Agreeableness,
Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness—as well as four narrow
personality traits: Sense of Identity, Optimism, Tough-Mindedness, and Work Drive.
Results of multiple regression analyses indicated that, after controlling for age, year in
school, and sex, the Big Five traits accounted for 37% of the variance in learner selfdirection,
with other narrow traits accounting for an additional 15% variance. A
combination of six Big Five and other narrow traits account for over 52% of the
variance in learner self-direction. It was suggested that other personality traits may be
influencing learner self-direction. Results are discussed in terms of theoretical and
methodological implications.
This paper addresses the relationship between learner self-direction and other
personality traits of college students when the traits represented by the Big Five model
are differentiated from narrow personality traits. Our study draws on and extends the
work of Lounsbury, Levy, Park, Gibson, and Smith (2009) who reported on the
development of a personality measure of learner self-direction and construct validity.
Before turning to their findings, we consider why this is an important topic.
Self-direction in learning is a major topic in the field of adult learning. There
has been extensive coverage of the topic by theorists, researchers, and practitioners
(Brockett & Hiemstra, 1991). Long (2007) has identified several themes and
measurements of self-direction in learning including psychological factors. There
have been several empirical measures created to look at different dimensions of selfdirection
in learning which address psychological factors such as the Self-Directed
Learning Readiness Scale (SDLRS) (Guglielmino, 1978), and more recently the Oddi
Continuing Learning Inventory (OCLI) (Oddi, 1986), and the Personal Responsibility
Orientation to Self-Direction in Learning Scale (PRO-SDLS) (Stockdale, 2003).
Research has shown that psychological variables are directly related to learner selfSelf-
Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
2
directedness (Oliveira & Simões, 2006). However, there have been few studies that
look at learner self-direction specifically in relation to personality traits.
In the rationale for their study, Lounsbury et al. (2009) made three important
observations:
1. Personality traits may influence or provide the foundation for selfdirection
in learning-development processes.
2. When considered as a whole, much of the prior literature on the
relationship between self-direction in learning and personality traits
(Johnson, Sample, & Jones, 1988; Leitsch & Van Hove, 1998) is
fragmented and piecemeal.
3. The Big Five model of personality represents an organizing scheme for
understanding self-direction in learning-personality trait relations.
With regard to the latter point, the Big Five model of personality traits of
Conscientiousness, Openness, Agreeableness, Extraversion, and Neuroticism (which
we will refer to by its inverse—Emotional Stability) is widely accepted as a unified,
parsimonious model of normal personality that has been validated in many different
cultures and across several research settings (De Raad, 2000; Digman, 1997), with
supporting studies based on many different demographic and personal characteristics
of individuals (Costa & McCrae, 1994).
The results of the Lounsbury et al. (2009) study indicated that there was a
significant relationship between the five-factor model of personality and learner selfdirection.
Their findings are important in that they further elucidate the nomological
network for learner self-direction; in this case, that self-directed students displayed
higher levels of Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness as well as lower
levels of Neuroticism. These results also provide empirical support for self-direction
in learning theorists who discuss the importance of such factors as creative
achievements, new experience, and student participation in learning projects, intrinsic
learning motivation, and self-concept (Hassan, 1982; Reynolds, 1986).
Drawing on recent developments in personality research, it is possible to extend
the work of Lounsbury et al. (2009) to other personality traits that go beyond the Big
Five model. Research in a number of areas has shown that validity can be enhanced
above and beyond the Big Five traits by considering more narrow personality traits,
which are defined as either subscales of the Big Five or as traits not encompassed by
the Big Five model. For example, Lounsbury, Sundstrom, Gibson, and Loveland
(2003) found that aggression and Work Drive added substantial variance to the
prediction of academic performance of middle and high school students beyond the
Big Five traits. Paunonen and Nicol (2001) found that narrow traits, such as selfdiscipline,
straightforwardness, and modesty, added significant incremental variance
beyond the Big Five when predicting 12 different criteria, including grade point
average, blood donations, absenteeism, and traffic violations. Also, Paunonen and
Ashton (2001) found that NEO Conscientiousness-related subscales of achievement,
self-discipline, competence, and dutifulness as well as the Openness-related subscale
of ideas added significantly to the prediction of collegiate GPA above and beyond the
Jackson Personality Inventory Conscientiousness scale. Accordingly, the purpose of
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
3
the present study was to investigate whether other narrow personality traits are related
to learner self-direction and to see if they contributed incremental validity to the
prediction of learner self-direction above and beyond the Big Five. The narrow traits
we examined were Sense of Identity, Optimism, Tough-Mindedness, and Work Drive.
These traits are not part of current Big Five taxonomies and have been found to be
related to important outcome criteria for college students including grades,
satisfaction, and intention to withdraw from school (cf. Lounsbury, Sadaurgas, &
Gibson, 2004; Lounsbury, Sadaurgas, Gibson, & Leong, 2005).
In the present study, our focus was on learner self-direction as an individual
differences variable that can be represented on a continuum from low to high. We
were not interested in representing learner self-direction as a categorical or nominal
variable representing an identity status such as state of foreclosure, diffusion,
moratorium, or achievement. With respect to Brockett and Hiemstra’s (1991) twodimension,
self-direction in learning taxonomy, our learner self-direction construct
corresponds to their learner self-direction construct. Consistent with prior
conceptualizations of self-direction in learning (Brockett, 1983; Brockett & Hiemstra,
1991; Costa & Kalick, 2003), we conceptualized and measured learner self-direction
as a personality construct reflecting an individual’s preference to be in charge of their
learning process; ability to conceptualize, plan, implement, and evaluate their
academic experience; and disposition to be goal-oriented and to work independently or
in group settings with little guidance.
We chose to study personality-learner self-direction relationships among college
students for several reasons. The college experience is regarded as providing “many
opportunities for students to develop, among other things, personal and professional
identity” (Hamrick, Evans, & Schuh, 2002, p. 135). As Madison (1969) observed,
college represents a unique and highly appropriate setting for studying Identity.
Moreover, for those individuals who go to college directly from high school, the
college experience occurs during a key developmental period for Identity development
(Waterman, 1985, 1993), and it is regarded as playing a “critical role in identity
formation” (Nakula, 2003, p. 9). We examined three research questions:
1. How much of the variance in learner self-direction can be accounted for jointly
by the Big Five traits?
2. Are the narrow traits of Sense of Identity, Optimism, Tough-Mindedness, and
Work Drive related to learner self-direction?
3. Do the narrow traits add incremental validity beyond the Big Five traits in
predicting learner self-direction?
Method
Participants
A total of 2102 students enrolled in an introductory psychology course and a
First-Year Studies program, at a large, public southeastern U. S. state university
volunteered to participate in this study. Demographic characteristics of the sample
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
4
were: Gender–68% female (32% male); Year in School–79% freshmen, 15%
sophomore, 3% junior, 3% senior; Age–3% under 18, 81% 18-19, 8% 20-21, 3% 22-
25, 2% 26-30, and 3% over 30.
Procedure
After obtaining human subjects approval from the university’s Institutional
Review Board, participants were solicited to take a personality inventory on-line.
Upon completion of the inventory, participants were provided a feedback report
summarizing their personality characteristics and implications for a variety of areas
related to being a student, including area of study, social life, managing stress, study
habits, living situation, and using campus resources. Students in the introductory
psychology course were offered extra credit for participation. All data were collected
between September 1, 2004 and December 30, 2004.
Personality Measure
The personality measure used in this study was the Resource Associates’
Transition to College inventory (RATTC). The RATTC is a normal personality
inventory contextualized for late adolescents (Jaffe, 1998) and adults through high
school and college. It measures the Big Five Traits of Agreeableness,
Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, Extraversion, and Openness as well as other
“narrow” personality traits and learner self-direction. Scale development, norming,
reliability, criterion-related validity, and construct validity information for the RATTC
can be found in Lounsbury and Gibson (2010).
Findings from the above studies demonstrated that the RATTC constructs are
internally consistent and display generally high convergence with common traits on
other, widely used personality inventories, including the 16 PF, NEO-PI-R, and the
Myers-Briggs Type Inventory (e.g., the RATTC measure of Extraversion correlates
.77 with NEO-PI-R measure of Extraversion). Moreover, the Big Five measures of the
RATTC significantly predict collegiate academic performance and withdrawal
intention (Lounsbury, Sundstrom, Gibson, & Loveland, 2003; Ridgell & Lounsbury,
2004). An adult version of the RATTC has been found to be related to job
performance, job satisfaction, and career satisfaction in a wide variety of occupations
in many different business and industry settings (Lounsbury & Gibson, 2010).
Big Five and narrow traits assessed. The Big Five and narrow traits measured
in this study, along with brief descriptions and their coefficient alphas, are listed
below:
• Agreeableness: being agreeable, participative, helpful, cooperative, and
inclined to interact with others harmoniously (coefficient alpha = .81)
• Conscientiousness: being conscientious, reliable, trustworthy, orderly, and
rule-following (coefficient alpha = .78)
• Emotional Stability: overall level of adjustment and emotional resilience in the
face of stress and pressure. We conceptualized this as the inverse of
Neuroticism (coefficient alpha = .83)
• Extraversion: tendency to be sociable, outgoing, gregarious, warmhearted,
expressive, and talkative (coefficient alpha = .84)
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
5
• Openness: receptivity and Openness to change, innovation, new experience,
and learning (coefficient alpha = .76)
• Sense of Identity: knowing one’s self and where one is headed in life, having a
core set of beliefs and values that guide decisions and actions; and having a
sense of purpose (coefficient alpha = .77)
• Optimism: having an optimistic, hopeful outlook concerning prospects, people,
and the future, even in the face of difficulty and adversity as well as a tendency
to minimize problems and persist in the face of setbacks (coefficient alpha =
.83)
• Tough-Mindedness: appraising information and making decisions based on
logic, facts, and data rather than feelings, sentiments, values, and intuition
(coefficient alpha = .75)
• Work Drive: being hard-working, industrious, and inclined to put in long hours
and much time and effort to reach goals and achieve at a high level (coefficient
alpha = .85)
Learner self-direction items. The ten items comprising the learner selfdirection
subscale of the Resource Associates Transition to College (RATTC)
inventory are listed below. Item responses were made on a five-point Likert scale:
1=Strongly Disagree; 2= Disagree; 3=Neutral/Undecided; 4=Agree; 5=Strongly
Agree.
1. I regularly learn things on my own outside of class.
2. I am very good at finding out answers on my own for things that the
teacher does not explain in class.
3. If there is something I don’t understand in a class, I always find a way to
learn it on my own.
4. I am good at finding the right resources to help me do well in school.
5. I view self-directed learning based on my own initiative as very important
for success in school and in my future career.
6. I set my own goals for what I will learn.
7. I like to be in charge of what I learn and when I learn it.
8. If there is something I need to learn, I find a way to do so right away.
9. I am better at learning things on my own than most students.
10. I am very motivated to learn on my own without having to rely on other
people.
For the present sample, the coefficient alpha for the above RATTC was .85.
Demographic Variables
The age and gender of students were assessed using categorical items. In
addition, we used two nontraditional student subgroups provided by the Nontraditional
Student Resource Guide (University of Oregon, 2005) to ask respondents whether
either of these characteristics applied to them:
• Over the age of 25
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
6
• Returning to or starting college after a long break
Results
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among the study variables are
displayed in Table 1. All of the Big Five personality traits are correlated significantly
and positively with learner self-direction, except for Extraversion. Specifically, in
descending order of magnitude, the correlations with learner self-direction were:
Openness (r = .43, p < .01), Emotional Stability (r = .20, p < .01), Conscientiousness
(r = .20, p < .01), Agreeableness (r = .21, p < .01), Extraversion (r = .01, p > .01),
and the other narrow personality traits also correlated significantly with learner selfdirection,
with the largest magnitude correlation observed for Work Drive (r = .49, p <
.01), followed by Optimism (r = .31, p < .01), Sense of Identity (r = .30, p < .01), and
Tough-Mindedness (r = -.07, p < .05).
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations for the Personality and
Satisfaction Variables
___________________________________________________________________________________
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10)
(1) Agreeableness — .16 .28 .02 .19 .34 .33 -.31 .26 .21
(2) Conscientiousness — .13 .06 .05 .28 .23 -.11 .33 .20
(3)Emotional Stability — .24 .07 .46 .59 .14 .09 .20
(4) Extraversion — .01 .26 .34 -.15 -.01 .01
(5) Openness — .21 .18 -.16 .41 .43
(6) Sense of Identity — .67 -.22 .36 .30
(7) Optimism — -.16 .26 .31
(8)Tough-Mindedness — -.23 -.07
(9) Work Drive — .49
(10) SDL —
Mean 3.74 3.38 3.17 3.54 3.52 3.96 4.01 2.32 3.18 3.29
Standard Deviation .62 .50 .69 .66 .59 .62 .57 .65 .62 .59
___________________________________________________________________________
Note: n = 2102; medial effect size = 25.5; range of effect size = -.07 to .49
Correlations > .09 or < -.09 are significant at the p < .01 level.
Correlations > .05 and < .09 or < -.05 and > -.09 are significant at the p < .05 level.
To evaluate research questions 1 and 3, we performed a series of regression
analyses with learner self-direction serving as the criterion variable and three
demographic variables which have been linked to Identity—age, sex, and year in
school (which in the present study correlated .11 (p < .01), .14 (p < .01), and .05 (p <
.05), respectively, with learner self-direction)—serving as control variables by
entering them as a set on the first step of each regression analysis. In the first analysis,
the Big Five traits were regressed on learner self-direction in stepwise fashion and all
five significantly entering the equation, accounting for 37% of the variance in learner
self-direction beyond the 3 demographic variables, as can be seen in the first
regression result in Table 2.
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
7
Table 2. Regression Analysis for Learner Self-Direction with Age, Year in School,
and Gender Entered First Followed by the Big Five Traits Entered Stepwise
Step Variable(s) Multiple R R2 R2 Change
1 Age, year in school, and gender .172** .030** .030**
2 Emotional Stability .506** .256** .226**
3 Conscientiousness .563** .317** .061**
4 Extraversion .586** .343** .026**
5 Openness .603** .362** .020**
6 Agreeableness .608** .370** .007**
Note: n = 2102 * p <.05 ** p <.01
To answer the question of whether the four narrow traits contributed incremental
variance in the prediction of learner self-direction beyond the Big Five, the following
regression procedure was employed. The three demographic variables were entered as
a set hierarchically on the first step, followed by the set of Big Five traits on the
second step; the narrow traits were allowed to enter in stepwise fashion. As can be
seen in the results in Table 3, the demographic and Big Five variables accounted for
37% of the variance, with Optimism adding an additional 14% of the variance (p <
.01), and Work Drive contributing an additional 1.5% of the variance (p < .01) in
learner self-direction. Sense of Identity and Tough-Mindedness did not account for
any significant variance in learner self-direction.
Table 3. Regression Analysis for Learner Self-Direction with Age, Year in School, And
Gender Entered First, The Big Five Traits Entered Second as A Set, Followed by
Narrow Traits Entered Stepwise
Step Variable(s) Multiple R R2 R2 Change
1 Age, year in school, and sex .172** .030** .030**
2 Big five traits .608** .370** .340**
3 Optimism .717** .514** .144**
4 Work Drive .727** .529** .015**
Note: n = 2102 * p <.05 ** p <.01
As can be seen in the third regression results in Table 4, when the Big Five and
narrow traits were allowed to enter the regression in stepwise fashion after the
demographic variables, Optimism entered first, contributing an additional 44% of the
variance (p < .01); Work Drive entered next, adding 3% (p < .01), followed by
Conscientiousness (R2-change = 1.4%, p < .01), Emotional Stability (R2-change =
.008%, p < .01), and Tough-Mindedness (R2-change = .005%, p < .01). These five
personality traits jointly accounted for over 50% of the variance in learner selfdirection
beyond that accounted for by the demographic variables of age, year in
school, and gender.
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
8
Table 4. Stepwise Regression Analysis for Learner Self-Direction with Age, Year in
School, and Gender Entered First; Then all Personality Traits Entered Stepwise
Step Variable(s) Multiple R R2 R2 Change
1 Age, Year in School, and
Gender
.172** .030** .030**
2 Optimism .689** .474** .444**
3 Work Drive .711** .506** .032**
4 Conscientiousness .721** .519** .014**
5 Emotional Stability .727** .527** .008**
6 Tough-Mindedness .730** .532** .005**
Note: n = 2102 * p <.05 ** p <.01
Discussion
The findings of this study provide support for Lounsbury et al.’s (2009)
summary “…of the importance and richness of the self-directed learning construct and
provide strong support for its role as a personality trait…” (p. 417). All of the Big
Five traits correlated significantly with learner self-direction, except for Extraversion.
The significant, positive relationships between learner self-direction and Emotional
Stability are consistent with Lounsbury, et al.’s correlational findings of a negative
relationship between Neuroticism and learner self-direction. Moreover, the results of
the present study indicate that the Big Five traits jointly explained a substantial
amount of variance in learner self-direction, which provides additional support for the
robustness of the Big Five model (e.g., De Raad, 2000; McCrae & Costa, 1997, 2003).
It appears that additional variance in learner self-direction can also be accounted
for by other narrow personality traits. The results of the second regression analysis
indicate that the traits of Optimism and Work Drive added incremental variance
beyond the Big Five in predicting learner self-direction. Moreover, the results of
regression analysis indicate that the narrow traits of Optimism and Work Drive
entered the regression equation to predict learner self-direction before any of the Big
Five traits. At this stage of research development, we would not conclude that any one
of the personality traits studied is more strongly related to learner self-direction than
other traits, but the moderate magnitude of the Optimism–learner self-direction
correlation is noteworthy and would be a prime candidate for replication and
explication by future research. Consistent with recommendations in other research
domains to use multidimensional composites (Paunonen & Nicol, 2001; Schneider,
Hough, & Dunnette, 1996), comprised of both broad traits such as the Big Five and
narrow personality measures, to maximize validity, we suggest that future research on
the relationships between learner self-direction and other personality traits consider
both the full set of Big Five traits as well as narrow traits of interest which need not be
limited to the small number of narrow traits we considered.
The generalizability of other personality traits and learner self-direction across
different domains of demographic and social role characteristics augurs well for future
self-direction in learning theory development which seeks to establish generalized
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
2
9
construct relations involving personality traits, and it also provides food for thought
concerning a crucial unresolved issue noted by Clancy and Dollinger (1993) and
framed here as: What is the causal direction of other personality traits in relation to
learner self-direction? That is, do other personality traits influence or contribute to
learner self-direction, or does learner self-direction influence other personality traits,
or is the relationship bi-directional? Attempts to resolve this issue should involve a
longitudinal design, which was not utilized in either Lounsbury et al. (2009) or the
present investigation, and may involve measurement of college student experiences
and activities through which personality is manifested. As but one example, it may be
that higher levels of Conscientiousness and Work Drive lead to more successful study
habits and academic performance, which may, in turn, lead to higher levels of learner
self-direction.
Nevertheless, there are several considerations that point toward a conceptual
model emphasizing the primacy of personality traits and portraying personality traits
as leading to learner self-direction. From a lifespan-developmental perspective (e.g.,
Berger, 2001; Erickson, 1980) identity issues emerge primarily in adolescence,
whereas personality traits, including constructs corresponding to the Big Five, have
been reliably studied for children as young as age 3 (van Lieshout & Haselager, 1993,
1994); thus, it is not unreasonable to consider other personality traits as preceding
learner self-direction. Moreover, personality traits are typically regarded as being
relatively invariant or consistent over time and across situations and environmental or
situational characteristics (e.g., Pervin & John, 1997).
In view of the above, we suggest that if personality traits are relatively consistent
for students across situations and over time, and if learner self-direction changes more
across situations and over time, the most logical interpretation of why the personality
trait–learner self-direction relationship is relatively consistent within and across such
disparate factors as age and returning to college after a long break is because the
personality traits are driving the relationship, which implies that other personality
traits are affecting learner self-direction, not that learner self-direction is influencing
other personality traits. This is a conceptual model which should be more rigorously
evaluated by future research, but should it prove to be even partially true, it would
have major implications for those theories of self-direction in learning which place
primary emphasis on the role of personal experiences and environmental determinants
of college student self-direction. Such a model would not rule out the role of
experiential and environmental factors in self-direction in learning for college
students; rather, it would mean that personality traits, even traits measured in high
school, may influence collegiate activities and experiences which may, in turn,
influence the learner self-direction of college students. It may be that personality
traits, not academic and personal experiences, are the major determinants of college
student self-direction in learning.
Directions for Future Research
There are a number of interesting areas for future research that could clarify and
extend the present findings. In addition to the need for replication on different
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
0
samples, research could be conducted on how the Big Five and narrow personality
traits relate to Sense of Identity and learner self-direction. Another topic to investigate
further is the relationship between age of college students and learner self-direction.
As mentioned earlier, perhaps the most important need for future research is to utilize
longitudinal research designs to help clarify the direction of causality for personality
traits vis-à-vis learner self-direction and to try to determine how these linkages are
established. For example, do individuals who are more optimistic engage in new
learning activities than more pessimistic individuals, with optimism helping to
facilitate self-direction? Hopefully, subsequent research in this area can assess the
linkages among learner self-direction, Big Five and narrow traits, and a variety of
important criteria in the college student domain, including grades (e.g., Furnham,
Chamorro-Premuzic, & McDougall, 2003; Lounsbury, Sundstrom, Loveland et al.,
2003), life satisfaction (Lounsbury et al., 2004), dropout (Heilbrun, 1962, 1965) life
satisfaction, and subjective well-being (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998).
Study Limitations
There are two primary limitations of the current study that should be
acknowledged. First, this study was limited to a four-month interval in time for a
single year in a single geographic area at a large, public university, leaving open the
question of generalizability to other time periods, geographic areas, and types of
universities. Second, most of the study participants were underclassmen; thus, we do
not know if the results would generalize to samples from other educational levels.
Conclusions
The results of the present study indicate that part of the Big Five traits as well as
three of the four other narrow traits measured in this study were each related to learner
self-direction, with other narrow traits adding incremental validity to the Big Five and
accounting for substantial variance in learner self-direction on their own. In
combination, the Big Five and narrow traits accounted for more than half of the
variance in learner self-direction and a composite of six traits was found to be
substantially related to learner self-direction for eight different subgroups of students
representing different categories of nontraditional students and student gender. Taken
as a whole, the present findings were interpreted as, in part, confirming and extending
the results of Lounsbury et al. (2009) regarding the Big Five and learner self-direction,
demonstrating the generalizability of personality trait-learner self-direction
relationships across a variety of different demographic and personal subgroups of
students and providing some clues that the direction of the causal arrow may be from
other personality traits to learner self-direction.
In conclusion, it is clear that learner self-direction has manifold connections to
other personality traits and is not clearly associated with just one of the Big Five traits.
In a sense, this pattern of multiple connections to personality is consistent with the
diverse factors learner self-direction has been linked to in the theoretical literature, as,
for example, the six vectors of college student development that Chickering and
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
1
Reisser (1993) posit as leading to identity establishment for college students.
Hopefully, further research will extend and clarify the nomological network of other
personality traits and learner self-direction.
References
Berger, K. S. (2001). The developing person through the life span (5th ed.). New
York, NY: Worth Publishers.
Brockett, R. (1983). Self-directed learning and the hard-to-reach adult. Lifelong
Learning: The Adult Years, 6(8), 16-18.
Brockett, R., & Hiemstra, R. (1991). Self-direction in adult learning: Perspectives on
theory, research, and practice. Retrieved from http://home.twcny.rr.com/
hiemstra/sdlindex.html
Chickering, A., & Reisser, L. (1993). Education and identity. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
Clancy, S. M., & Dollinger, S. J. (1993). Identity, self, and personality: Identity status
and the Five-Factor model of personality. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
3(3), 227-245.
Costa, P., & Kallick, B. (2003). Assessment strategies for self-directed learning.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1994). Stability and change in personality from adolescence
through adulthood. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin
(Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy
to adulthood (pp. 139-155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
De Raad, B. (2000). The big five personality factors: The psycholexical approach to
personality. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
DeNeve, K., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137
personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197-
229.
Digman, J. (1997). Higher-order factors of the big five. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 73, 1246-1256.
Erickson, E. H. (1980). Identity and the life cycle. New York, NY: Norton.
Furnham, A., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & McDougall, F. (2003). Personality, cognitive
ability, and beliefs about intelligence as predictors of academic performance.
Learning and Individual Differences, 14, 49-66.
Guglielmino, L. M. (1977). Development of the Self-Directed Learning Readiness
Scale. Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, 6467A.
Hamrick, F., Evans, N. J., & Schuh, J. H. (2002). Foundations of student affairs
practice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Hassan, A. M. (1982). An investigation of the learning projects among adults of high
and low readiness for self-direction in learning. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 42, 3838A.
Heilbrun, A. B. (1962). Prediction of first year college drop-out using ACL needs
scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 9(1), 58-63.
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
2
Heilbrun, A. B. (1965). Personality factors in college dropout. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 49(1), 1-7.
Jaffe, M. L. (1998). Adolescence. New York, NY: Wiley.
Johnson, J. A., Sample, J. A., & Jones, W. J. (1988). Self-directed learning and
personality type in adult degree students. Psychology: A Journal of Human
Behavior, 25(1), 32-36.
Leitsch, P. K., & Van Hove, S. D. (1998). Extraverted intuitives: A profile of adult
learners. Psychology: A Journal of Human Behavior, 35(3-4), 44-49.
Long, H. B. (2007). Themes and theses in self-directed learning. International Journal
of Self-Directed Learning, 4(2), 1-18. Retrieved from http://www.
sdlglobal.com
Lounsbury, J., & Gibson, L. (2010). Technical manual for the Resource Associates
Personal Style Inventory and Adolescent Personal Style Inventory. Knoxville,
TN: Resource Associates.
Lounsbury, J., Levy, J., Park, S., Gibson, L., & Smith, R. (2009). An investigation of
the construct validity of the personality trait of self-directed learning. Learning
and Individual Differences, 19, 411-418.
Lounsbury, J., Sadaurgas, R., & Gibson, L. (2004). An investigation of Big Five and
narrow personality traits in relation to intention to withdraw from college.
Journal of College Student Development, 45(5), 517-534.
Lounsbury, J., Sadaurgas, R., Gibson, L., & Leong, T. (2005). An investigation of
broad and narrow personality traits in relation to general and domain-specific
life satisfaction of college students. Research in Higher Education, 46(6), 707-
729.
Lounsbury, J., Sundstrom, E., Gibson, L., & Loveland, J. (2003). Broad versus narrow
personality traits in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Learning
and Individual Differences, 14(1), 65-75.
Lounsbury, J., Sundstrom, E., Loveland, J., & Gibson, L. (2003). Intelligence, “big
five” personality traits and work drive as predictors of course grade.
Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1231-1239.
Madison, P. (1969). Personality development during college. New York, NY:
Addison-Wesley.
McCrae, R., & Costa, P. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal.
American Psychologist, 52, 509-516.
McCrae, R., & Costa, P. (2003). Personality in adulthood: A five-factor theory
perspective (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Nakula, M. (2003). Identity and possibility. In M. Sadowski (Ed.), Adolescents at
school: Perspectives on youth, identity, and education. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard.
Oddi, L. F. (1986). Development and validation of an instrument to identify selfdirected
continuing learners. Adult Education Quarterly, 36, 97-107.
Oliveira, A. L., & Simões, A. (2006). Impact of socio-demographic and psychological
variables on the self-directedness of higher education students. International
Journal of Self-Directed Learning, 3(1), 1-12. Retrieved from
http://www.sdlglobal.com
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
3
Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big Five factors and facets and the
prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1(3),
524-539.
Paunonen, S. V., & Nicol, A. A. (2001). The personality hierarchy and the prediction
of work behaviors. In F. Hogan & B. Roberts (Eds.), Personality psychology in
the workplace (pp. 161-191). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological
Association.
Pervin, L. A., & John, O. (1997). Personality: Theory and research. New York, NY:
Wiley.
Reynolds, M. M. (1986). The self-directedness and motivational orientations of adult
part-time students at a community college. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 38, 607-618.
Ridgell, S., & Lounsbury, J. (2004). Predicting collegiate academic success: General
intelligence, “Big Five” personality traits, and work drive. College Student
Journal, 38, 607-618.
Schneider, R. J., Hough, L. M., & Dunnette, M. D. (1996). Broadside by broad traits:
How to sink science in five dimensions or less. Journal of Organizational
Behavior, 17, 639-655.
Stockdale, S. L. (2003). Development of an instrument to measure self-directedness.
Dissertation Abstracts International, A64/06, AAT 3092836.
van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Haselager, G. J. T. (1993). The Big Five personality factors
in the Nijmegen California Child Q-set (NCCQ). Nijmegen, the Netherlands:
University of Nijmegan.
van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Haselager, G. J. T. (1994). The Big Five personality factors
in Q-sort descriptions of children and adolescents. In C. F. Halverson, G. A.
Kohnstamm & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament
and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 293-318). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Waterman, A. S. (1985). Identity in the context of adolescent psychology. In A. S.
Waterman (Ed.), Identity in adolescence: Process and contents (pp. 5-24). San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Waterman, A. S. (1993). Developmental perspectives on identity formation: From
adolescence to adulthood. In J. Marcia, A. Waterman, D. Matteson, S. Archer,
& J. Orlofsky (Eds.), Ego identity (pp. 42-68). New York, NY: Springer-
Verlag.
Jeral Kirwan (jkirwan@utk.edu) is a doctoral student in the Adult Learning Program
at the University of Tennessee, Knoxville. His research interests include self-direction
in learning, personality, and psychometrics.
John Lounsbury (jlounsbu@utk.edu) is President, Resource Associates, Inc., and Full
Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Tennessee. Dr. Lounsbury is a
Fellow of the American Psychological Association and a licensed
Industrial/Organizational Psychologist.
Self-Direction in Learning and Personality
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
4
Lucy Gibson is Vice President of Resource Associates, Inc. Dr. Gibson is a Licensed
Industrial/Organizational Psychologist who has many years of experience in the areas
of test development, test validation, and implementation of selection testing programs.
She has taught at the University of Tennessee and Tusculum College.
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
5
THE ROLE OF SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING IN THE WORK OF
COMMUNITY LEADERS
Leatrice Turlis Phares and Lucy Madsen Guglielmino
Abstract
This study was designed to examine self-directed learning readiness of volunteer
community leaders and to explore their use of self-directed learning in their
community leadership roles. The mean for this sample on Guglielmino’s (1978)
SDLRS (Learner Preference Assessment) was significantly higher than the general
population mean. In-depth interviews with 10 of the 131 subjects revealed extensive
self-directed learning contributing to the participants’ community leadership efforts
and a strong belief that ongoing learning is essential to perform well as a community
leader.
The increasing complexity of our society, our work, and expanding technology
place more demands on those who volunteer for community leadership roles. In 1996
Kotter stated, “. . . by any objective measure, the amount of significant, often
traumatic, change in organizations has grown tremendously in the past two decades”
(p. 3), and the change has continued to escalate. O’Connell (2006) notes, “The
problems of contemporary society are more complex, the solutions are more involved
and the satisfaction more obscure, but the basic ingredients to progress are still the
caring and the resolve to make things better” (p. 7). Community leadership is defined
as assisting the public and private non-profit sectors in meeting the changing needs of
local communities, organizations and citizens (Kouzes & Posner, 1995). Today’s
society expects its leaders to take the initiative and devise goals and strategies to solve
our increasingly complex community problems, working effectively both individually
and within groups. Clark (1999) asserts that leaders must be creative problem solvers
who work in a team atmosphere and are able to organize resources to accomplish tasks
with maximum efficiency. They need to be flexible, able to assess situations quickly
and accurately and to create appropriate goals. Kouzes and Posner (1995) surveyed
several thousand business and government executives and found that forward thinking
and a sense of direction were other important leadership characteristics; and Kotter
(1998) found that the most notable trait of great leaders is their quest for learning.
Voluntary community leaders step forward to take responsibility for community
problems, often with little or no formal preparation, gathering information and
marshalling resources to address new issues and challenges. The characteristics and
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
6
actions of community leaders described in the literature suggest that they are highly
self-directed learners.
Self-directed learning has been described as a process in which individuals
take the initiative, with or without the help of others, in diagnosing their learning
needs, formulating learning goals, identifying human and material resources for
learning, choosing and implementing appropriate learning strategies, and evaluating
learning outcomes (Knowles, 1975). Tough (1971) found that 98% of adults are
involved in self-planned learning, with a mean of 8.3 projects a year, each averaging
8.16 hours, and several recent studies have reinforced his findings (Davis, Bailey,
Nypaver, Rees, & Brockett, 2010; Guglielmino et al., 2005). According to Merriam,
Caffarella, and Baumgartner (2007), “Guglielmino has provided the most-used
operational definition for self-directed learning” (p. 121). Guglielmino (1978)
described a highly self-directed learner based on her Delphi study of experts in selfdirected
learning:
A highly self-directed learner is one who exhibits initiative, independence, and
persistence in learning; one who accepts responsibility for his or her own
learning and views problems as challenges, not obstacles; one who is capable
of self-discipline and has a high degree of curiosity; one who has a strong
desire to learn or change and is self-confident; one who is able to use basic
study skills, organize his or her own time, set an appropriate pace for learning,
and develop a plan for completing work; one who enjoys learning and has a
tendency to be goal-oriented. (p. 73)
A growing body of literature supports a link between self-directed learning and
attainment of or performance in leadership roles (Boyce, 2004; Durr, 1992; Connelly,
2004; Guglielmino, 1996; Guglielmino & Guglielmino, 1983, 2008; Kandarian, 2004;
Roberts, 1986). Self-Directed Learning Readiness Scale (SDLRS) levels are even
higher among top entrepreneurs in the U.S. who, like community leaders, often have
fewer guidelines such as corporate policies to guide or restrict their actions
(Guglielmino & Klatt, 1994).
Many leaders in the workplace, whether in business, education, or other areas,
are also community leaders. It appears that the processes of community problem
solving and self-directed learning are analogous, as are the characteristics of effective
community leaders and the characteristics of highly self-directed learners. Imel
(1999) states that there are those who participate in self-directed learning for the
process of community problem solving. However, the use of self-directed learning by
volunteer community leaders has not previously been investigated in depth. Taylor
(2002) raised the issue that “there is very little in the literature that analyzes exactly
how self-directed learning is happening, the dynamics of learning in these contexts or
the differences between learning as an individual for personal reasons and learning as
an individual member of a group working for a common cause” (p. 44). Determining
the levels of self-directed learning readiness of community leaders and exploring
whether they use self-directed learning in their leadership roles (and, if so, how) can
enhance our understanding of the process of community leadership and provide
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
7
valuable insights to improve the limited professional development available for
community leaders.
Purpose and Research Questions
This study was designed to examine the self-directed learning readiness of
community leaders and to explore their use of self-directed learning in their
community leadership roles. Three research questions and one hypothesis guided the
study:
1. What is the mean level of self-directed learning readiness of community leaders
and how does it compare to the readiness levels of other groups?
2. What types of learning projects did community leaders participate in during the 12-
month period prior to the study?
3. Did the community leaders use self-directed learning projects to carry out their
community leadership roles? If so, to what extent?
One quantitative research hypothesis was posed to investigate the first research
question. The second research question was explored through documentation and
analysis of learning projects in structured in-depth interviews. The third research
question was explored through analysis of the responses to open-ended questions
incorporated into the interview.
Null Hypothesis: There is no significant difference in the level of self-directed
learning readiness of community leaders as measured by the Self-Directed
Learning Readiness Scale (SDLRS) and the mean SDLRS score for U.S.
adults.
Delimitations and Limitations
This study was delimited to volunteer leaders of community service
organizations, specifically (a) board members of the Leadership Broward Foundation
in Fort Lauderdale, Florida; Leadership Miami in Miami, Florida; Leadership Palm
Beach County in West Palm Beach, Florida; and (b) Rotarians who have a leadership
role in Rotary District 6990 and live in Broward, Miami-Dade, and Monroe Counties
in Florida.
The convenience sample also constitutes a limitation. The primary researcher
is a member of two of the organizations studied, which creates an advantage in terms
of access to participants, but may affect the content of participant response and
interpretation of results. However, the researchers strove for objectivity and an
additional professional educator reviewed the transcripts and data analysis.
Method
To assess the readiness for self-direction in learning among the community
leaders and compare it to the means of other groups, the SDLRS (Guglielmino, 1978)
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
8
was used. A modification of Tough’s (1971) interview schedule was used to gather
data on the learning projects of a selected sample of the community leaders. Openended
questions were added to the interview to further explore the use of self-directed
learning in community leadership roles. The data collection instruments are described
below.
Instruments
Self-Directed Learning Readiness Scale. The SDLRS is the most often used
quantitative measure of self-directed learning (Merriam, Caffarella, & Baumgartner,
2007). It is a 58-item, five-point Likert-type scale that measures the attitudes, values,
and abilities of learners relating to their readiness to engage in self-directed learning at
the time of their response. This readiness is assessed as a total score, which is then
converted into bands of high, above average, average, below average and low levels
of readiness (Guglielmino & Guglielmino, 1991). The SDLRS is referred to in test
settings as the Learning Preference Assessment (LPA). The reliability has most often
been assessed through measures of internal consistency; estimates normally range
from .87-.92 (Delahaye & Choy, 2000).
Content validity of the SDLRS was established by Guglielmino’s (1978)
development process, which used a modified Delphi technique that involved a panel of
experts in three rounds of surveys to identify the characteristics of a highly selfdirected
learner. Fourteen researchers known for their work in the area of selfdirection
in learning participated. Among them were Malcolm Knowles, Allen Tough,
Cyril Houle, B. Frank Brown, Arthur W. Chickering, Wilbert J. McKeachie, and
Morris Weitman (Guglielmino, 1997). The vast majority of studies have supported the
reliability and validity of the instrument (for example, Chuprina & Durr, 2006;
Connolly, 2004; Delahaye & Smith, 1995; Durr, 1992; Finestone, 1984; Graeve, 1987;
Hassan, 1981; Liddell, 2007; Long & Agyekum, 1984; McCune, 1988; McCune &
Guglielmino, 1991; Muller, 2007; Oliviera & Simões, 2006; Posner, 1989-90; Zsiga,
2007). There has been some criticism (Brockett, 1985; Field, 1989), responded to by
Long (1989), McCune (1989), and Guglielmino (1989). A comprehensive review by
Delahaye and Choy (2000) concluded, “There has been extensive support for the
[SDLRS] LPA in the literature as an accurate and useful instrument for measuring
readiness for self-directed learning” (p. 2).
Tough’s interview schedule with additional open-ended questions. A
modification of Tough’s (1971) interview schedule was used to answer the research
questions regarding the types of learning projects the community leaders had
participated in over the 12 months preceding their interviews. Numerous studies using
Tough’s Interview Schedule have been conducted (for example, Brasfield, 1984,
Coolican, 1975; Davis et al., 2010; Estrin, 1986; Guglielmino et al., 2005; Hiemstra,
1976; Penland, 1978, 1979; Peters & Gordon, 1974; Ralston, 1981). Although there
have been variations in both the total number of learning projects and in the total
percentage of self-planned projects, the findings from the original Tough investigation
have largely been substantiated (Brockett & Hiemstra, 1991).
Modification of the interview schedule for this study involved the addition of
open-ended questions. One broad question was added specifically to explore learning
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
3
9
projects related to the performance of the subjects’ community leadership roles: “Let’s
take a little time now to talk specifically about learning and your community
leadership role. Would you tell me about any new learning required for your
community leadership role?” Appropriate follow-up questions were asked to fully
explore this topic.
Procedures
Assessment of SDL readiness. The convenience sample for the assessment of
readiness for self-directed learning consisted of volunteers who held leadership roles
in community organizations. The participants were recruited from the specified groups
until a usable sample size was obtained. Power analysis indicated that at least 128
subjects were needed to obtain a power of .80 with a medium effect size (d = .50) with
an alpha of .05.
After approval by the Institutional Review Board, the primary researcher
requested letters of support for the study from leaders of the targeted organizations and
then contacted the executive directors of Leadership Broward Foundation, Leadership
Miami, and Leadership Palm Beach County to gain permission to attend a board
meeting and explain the research study. After explaining the study, she distributed the
data collection material to board members who agreed to participate at that time. The
materials included: (a) Institutional Review Board consent forms, (b) the Learning
Preference Assessment (SDLRS), (c) the demographic form, and (d) a self-addressed,
stamped envelope to return the completed questionnaire.
The researcher used the District 6990 Rotary International 2004-2005 Official
Directory as a guide to identify the Rotarians in a District Rotary 6990 leadership role
(Benson, 2004). She then attended major Rotary District 6990 events and asked
Rotary leaders if they would like to volunteer to complete the LPA questionnaire.
Procedures for administration were the same as described for the leadership groups,
except that these participants were offered the option to complete the questionnaire
and demographic sheet at that time or return it to the researcher at a later date. The
materials were distributed to 172 potential participants. The primary researcher placed
a follow-up telephone call to participants who had not returned their questionnaires
within two weeks. Seventy-one participants chose to return the material by mail, while
60 participants completed the material onsite, resulting in a 76% response rate for the
survey (131 of 172).
Exploration of learning projects. Once the LPA forms were administered and
the completed forms were returned, the researcher chose a subsample of ten
community leaders to represent a cross-section of ethnicity, gender, age, education,
and the four identified organizations. SDLRS (LPA) scores were not computed before
the individuals were selected and interviewed. The ten interviews were based on a
modification of Tough’s Interview Schedule to obtain direct information about the
types of learning projects the community leaders participated in over the 12 months
preceding the interviews.
The interviews, conducted by the primary researcher, lasted from one to two
hours; they took place at locations mutually agreed upon by the researcher and the
participants. Participants were assured of the confidentiality of their responses and
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
0
their right to withdraw at any time and were asked to sign a consent form. All
participants were asked the same questions in the same order, using both fixed choice
and open-ended questions. The open-ended questions were designed to explore in
detail the learning projects of the community leaders, and a final open-ended question
was added in an attempt to determine if and how community leaders used learning
projects to carry out their community leadership role. If early responses indicated that
they did, follow-up questions were used in an attempt to determine the extent to which
this had occurred. The participants’ replies were audiotaped and transcribed by the
researcher. To ensure accuracy of the acquired data, the researcher also used member
checking, asking the participants to review their transcripts for accuracy and make any
appropriate changes (Glesne, 1999).
Profile of Respondents to the LPA
There were 71 males and 60 females in the study. The majority (81.7%) of the
participants were Caucasian, with 9% African American, 6.1 % Hispanic, 1.5% Asian
or Pacific Islander and 0.8% American Indian or Alaskan Native. Most were between
the ages of 36 and 65, with the largest number being 56 – 65 years old. Everyone had
at least some college, with most having some graduate education. Almost all reported
that their employment level was professional or managerial.
Data Analysis
The completed SDLRS/LPA questionnaires were scored using the instructions
provided by the author (Guglielmino & Guglielmino, 1991) and the mean score was
calculated. The LPA mean score of the community leaders was compared to the
general adult population mean score of 214, to the mean of a meta-analytic study of
research using the LPA with adults in 29 different studies from 1977-1987 (McCune,
Guglielmino, & Garcia, 1990), and to a sample of top entrepreneurs (Guglielmino &
Klatt, 1994). T-tests were used for the comparisons. The quantitative items from
Tough’s Interview Schedule were tallied and the data described to develop an
understanding of the extent and types of learning projects conducted by community
leaders. In analyzing the open-ended questions that were added, the researchers
identified the most common responses and documented them with supporting
quotations. Two researchers independently conducted the analysis.
Findings
Self-Directed Learning Readiness
Mean score of sample. The mean LPA score of the sample of 131 participants
was 245.09 with a standard deviation of 19.04. The lowest score was 187 and the
highest score was 285. According to the conversion table (Guglielmino &
Guglielmino, 1991), the participants’ mean score of 245.09 ranked in the 87th
percentile and converted into a readiness level of above average. No participants
scored in the low readiness level. Two participants scored in the below average level
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
1
and 20 participants scored in the average level. Eighty-three percent (83%) of the
participants scored in the above average and high levels. Fifty-four scored in the above
average level and 55 scored in the high level.
Hypothesis testing. The null hypothesis tested in this study was that there is
no significant difference in the levels of the self-directed learning readiness of
community leaders as measured by the Self-Directed Learning Readiness Scale
(SDLRS/LPA) and the mean score for US adults and specified groups. The LPA mean
score of community leaders (M = 245.09, SD = 19.04) was significantly higher than
the LPA mean score of the general population (M = 214, SD = 25.59), t(130) = 18.69,
p<.001. The Cohen’s d for this comparison was 1.21, a large effect size (Cohen,
1988). The LPA mean score of community leaders was also significantly higher than
the LPA mean score of adults in 29 different studies from 1977-1987 (M = 227.7),
t(130) = 10.46, p<.001. The eta squared for this comparison was 0.45699, a large
effect size (Cohen, 1988). Finally, the LPA mean score of community leaders was
significantly lower than the LPA mean score of top entrepreneurs (M = 248.6), SD =
18.74), t(130) = 3.51, p =.037. The Cohen’s d for this comparison was 0.18, a small
effect size (Cohen, 1988). Consequently, the null hypothesis was not accepted. These
results support the alternative hypothesis that mean LPA scores for community leaders
differ from mean LPA scores of the general population, adults in 29 different studies
from 1977-1987, and top entrepreneurs.
Interviews
Profile of interview participants. Six males and 4 females completed Tough’s
Interview Schedule. All had previously completed the SDLRS (LPA) for this study and
were purposely chosen to be representative of the volunteer community leaders. The
researcher based the selection on community leadership organization, ethnicity, age
and education. The majority of the participants (70%) were Caucasian; the others were
African American (10%), Asian or Pacific Islander (10%) and Hispanic (10%). There
were an equal number of participants between the ages of 46 – 55 (30%) and the ages
of 56 – 65 (30%). There were also an equal number of participants between the ages of
36 – 45 (20%) and 66 – 75 (20%). All were college graduates and 40% had some
graduate education. All described their employment level as professional or
managerial.
Number of learning projects. The 10 participants completed an average of
16.2 learning projects during the previous year that met Tough’s (1971) seven-hour
minimum. The median number of learning projects was 15. The time spent on each
learning project averaged 123.1 hours, with a range from 10 hours to 2,000 hours.
Content of learning projects. The subjects of this study participated in a
variety of learning projects during the 12 months before their interviews. After the
interviews were completed, the researcher reviewed all the individual projects and
combined similar types of subject matter, identifying five main content categories:
employment / job-related, community organizations, personal interests, computers/
technology, and current events. Aspects of these categories relating to community
leadership roles are discussed; the personal interest category is omitted in this paper.
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
2
Employment / Job-related. The largest number of learning projects that the
participants identified related to their primary employment. All the participants had
more than one learning project that was job-related. Although most included or began
with formal learning settings, almost all included some form of self-directed learning
as a part of the learning project. The topics were varied and reflected the participants’
different types of employment, but many of their work-related projects also enhanced
their community leadership skills; for example, marketing, financial management,
survey methods, funding for public transportation, legal issues, and tax issues.
Community organizations. The second largest number of learning projects
centered on the leaders’ work community organizations. All participants identified
learning projects related to their work with community organizations. The subjects of
learning projects were varied and related to the needs of the individual community
leader and the organization. There were far fewer references to the inclusion of formal
learning segments in the learning projects related to community organizations as
compared to employment / job-related projects. Sample quotes are included later in
this paper.
Computers /Technology. All ten participants identified some type of computer
or technology-related learning project that was self-directed. These learning projects
focused on improving their skills, such as learning how to do PowerPoint
presentations, how to conduct Internet searches, and how to use new technology.
Learning about computers and technology was viewed as a tool to support other
learning. As one leader commented, “Internet research is probably one of the fastest
ways to educate yourself on a given subject and I found that ability, that experience
very vital in the new job that I have had.”
Current events. Seventy percent of the participants reported that current events
were an ongoing learning project for them. They read the newspapers, read the news
online, watched the news reports and shows on TV, followed the stock market, and
attended meetings that involved local government issues. One participant said, “I’ve
always had an interest in current events and so I just make it part of my day. Current
events are just something that’s a part of life.”
Learning projects in relation to community leadership roles. All
participants had voluntarily identified learning projects that related to their community
leadership roles before they were asked the final question, “Would you tell me about
any new learning required in your community leadership role?” When asked, they all
referred to previously-identified learning projects that related to their leadership of
community organizations. These were strongly represented in the community
organization, computer/technology, and current events categories and, to a lesser
extent, in the job-related category. The only one of the major categories that did not
appear to contribute meaningfully to learning for community leadership role was the
personal interest category.
As the researchers reviewed and analyzed the interviewees’ descriptions of
learning related to their community leadership roles, three concepts were mentioned
most often:
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
3
1. The community leaders had a desire to learn more about their organizations.
2. They identified self-directed learning that contributed to helping them do their
jobs better.
3. They recognized a need for their learning to be ongoing.
Desire to learn more about the organization. The two organizations from which
the subjects were selected were community-based, and the participants had been
elected to unpaid leadership roles within them. Some participants knew more about
their organizations than others, but they all expressed a desire to learn more. Two
sample quotes:
Well, as incoming president, what I set out to learn in greater detail was all
about our programs and the specifics of those, how we put those on, so that I
had a thorough understanding of what our organization is all about. And also, I
felt it very important that I better understand our budget so I spent a lot of time
digging behind the finances.
I had to learn a great deal about the organization that I was attempting to be the
leader of — a club that was one component of a huge international organization.
So I had to learn as much as I could about it. I attended conferences as well as
read the magazine and all the various materials provided by them. I did many
things on my own, ongoing learning.
Identification of learning that contributed to doing their jobs better as
community leaders. The participants identified many examples of learning that
contributed to doing their jobs better as community leaders. Most of the comments
reflected independent learning; others involved or grew out of group experiences.
I tried to learn a bit more about community water projects because potable
water is a great interest, and I knew I was going to attempt a matching grant
project on potable water.
I learned how to put on a web-based zone membership seminar, which had
never been done before. The web-based portion of it was motivated because
we have such a diverse zone.
Right now we are going out and learning different marketing aspects, different
techniques to get people interested in giving to the capital campaign.
I tried to learn how to do fundraising… for nonprofits to generate more
revenues for the projects that I am working with, to be more effective with
what I am already involved in.
I have been learning how to get volunteers to work together as a group and as
individuals.
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
4
Right now I’m going through a lot of training that details how to motivate
people to accomplish goals, how to grow membership, how to grow projects.
[I] went online to research resources on membership.
You get a lot of materials. [The Rotary] Manual [of] Procedures,
manuals of how to set up committees, manuals of all sorts of things.
Those are all the readings, books, pamphlets, and so forth.
I did many things on my own, but the conferences, two main
conferences I attended, the District and International, helped as well.
Being able to attend [conferences] and talk to people who can increase
my own understanding of what issues there are, whether they’re social
or monetary or environmental or professional, plays a big role in my
decision making.
It wasn’t so much a class as it was a commitment to chair a committee that
would stretch me as an individual–that would force me to spend more time
learning all there was to learn.
Ongoing learning. Most of the participants identified the learning as
“ongoing,” some using that exact word. Sample quotes illustrate their strong
expression of the need for ongoing learning:
Ongoing. Ongoing because the leaders are very helpful to one another. So it is
a constant process.
I believe that I need to continue to learn so when I’m making a judgment call,
I’m making judgments based upon experience both personally and from others
and also from knowledge that I gained from the various resources that I have
been able to use.
I did many things on my own, ongoing learning.
I’m continuing to school myself.
I read a considerable amount online. I’m constantly using different
reference sites and a considerable amount of news sites per day. . . .
So, I’m constantly reading.
I don’t think that you are ever through learning. There is always something
else to learn.
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
5
Conclusions and Discussion
Based on the findings of this study, three major conclusions were drawn:
community leaders are highly self-directed learners; they make extensive use of their
learning projects in their community leadership roles; and they recognize that their
community leadership roles require ongoing learning.
Community Leaders as Self-Directed Learners
Based on the sample studied, community leaders are highly self-directed
learners, as indicated by both their SDLRS scores and their involvement in learning
projects. The SDLRS mean score of community leaders (245.09) was higher than the
adult population mean (214) and higher than the mean of a large meta-analysis of 29
studies (227.7), but not as high as the mean of top entrepreneurs (248.6) in the U.S.
The numbers of learning projects undertaken by the community leaders and their
duration (discussed in the next section) similarly reflect a high level of self-direction
in learning.
It seems logical that high-level community leaders would be highly self-directed
learners because of the complex demands for learning that community leaders face. In
addition, this finding parallels the findings of high levels of self-directed learning
readiness among leaders in business. Another possible reason for the higher mean
score as compared to the adult population and the large meta-analytic study means
could be that the participants in the present study all had a least some college and most
were college graduates. Some previous studies have documented a relationship
between SDLRS means and educational level (for example, Durr, 1992; Roberts,
1986); however, some have not (Bryan-Wunner, 1991).
All of the participants had listed employment level as professional or
managerial. Studies by Roberts (1986) and Durr (1992) had shown that there is a
significant relationship between the SDLRS scores and participants’ managerial level
and management performance in large businesses; however, Bryan-Wunner (1991) did
not find significant differences in SDLRS scores of different levels of park and
recreation leaders. It was understandable that the participants scored lower than the top
entrepreneurs in the U.S. (Guglielmino & Klatt, 1994). In that study, a very select
group of 50 top entrepreneurs selected by a professional magazine constituted the
sample.
The results of this study add support to Brockett and Hiemstra’s (1991)
statement that self-direction in learning is clearly not limited to white, middle class
adults. Although the education level included some college for all participants, the
sample of community leaders in this study represented a variety of ethnicities. The
study sample included 107 Caucasians, 12 African Americans, 8 Hispanics, 2 Asians,
and 1 American Indian/Alaskan Native; and 3 of the 10 interviewees were non-
Caucasian.
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
6
Self-Directed Learning Projects of Community Leaders
There is evidence that community leaders make extensive use of self-directed
learning projects in carrying out their leadership roles.
Numbers. In the sample studied, the participants had completed an average of
16.2 learning projects during the past year that met Tough’s (1971) seven-hour
minimum. This was nearly twice the average number of projects reported in Tough’s
original findings (a mean of 8.3 learning projects in the previous year).
Time. The average number of hours spent on each project was 123.1 hours,
which was higher than Tough’s reported average of 100 hours per project. The hours
were also higher than the study by Guglielmino et al. (2005) that reported an average
of 56.1 hours per learning project. A few possible reasons for these differences could
be that the participants in this study were highly educated, all had professional or
managerial employment levels, and all were involved in more than one community
organization.
Reporting of learning projects related to community leadership. Of the five
major categories identified in all of the learning projects reported by community
leaders, learning projects relating to community leadership roles accounted for the
second highest number of projects. Other themes were employment/job related,
computer/technology, current events and personal interest. As could be expected,
employment/job related accounted for the largest number. All the participants had
identified and discussed learning projects that related to their community leadership
roles before being asked the final open-ended question. It appears that they were well
aware that they had participated in self-directed learning projects that helped them
carry out their leadership roles.
Emphasis on need for self-direction in learning for community leadership
roles. In addition, a comparison of the learning projects that were job-related as
compared to those relating to committee leadership roles revealed that many more of
those related to community leadership were completely self-planned and self-directed.
One interviewee’s comment suggested a reason for this strong difference. Mentioning
the “lack of direction” for community leaders, he commented:
You know, in most situations, you are given an assignment and you’re given
the expectation and a time line and so on. And you know the scope and the
magnitude of the job and then you can assess what you need to go about to
fulfilling that assignment. And then at the end of the day or the project, you’re
able to evaluate your progress. The problem with [community organizations] is
that they give you a title, and you ask, what’s the scope of the job? Well, the
job is to be in charge, so then you [ask], “Where’s my job description? What
are my duties? What do I have to do?” Well, don’t worry about it. You’ll do it
as you go.
The relative lack of specific job descriptions, training and formal guidelines
and procedures for community leadership roles places greater responsibility on the
individual to learn what it is needed to perform well, and these individuals took that
responsibility seriously. Two quotes summarize especially well the challenges faced
and commitment evidenced by community leaders as they try to make a difference:
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
7
It has been a tremendous growth experience. I have met some wonderful
people, locally and globally. It has forced me to really challenge myself to do
something. [I have] never done anything this tough, I don’t think, especially
nothing that I’ve done for free.
I was in a leadership position. I was obligated to learn as much as I could so
that I could share that knowledge with the group so we could become effective.
Emphasis on learning through conversation. It was interesting to note the
emphasis participants placed on talking with others in similar roles or those who had
expertise in the area they needed to learn about. One person described his learning
method as, “Meeting and talking.” Another remarked, “Networking put me in contact
with people who … were experts.” While nine of the interviewees reported that they
attended programs sponsored by the national offices of their local community
organizations, their comments regularly mentioned conversations with other
participants as a primary means of learning, as indicated by the following quote:
Attending the conference in Atlanta helped me understand what organizations
around the country are doing with respect to improving their communities and
try to take away ideas from that. . . You know, our organization has been
[around] for 25 years and you kind of get set in your ways. It’s refreshing to go
to these conferences and talk to other people and walk away with new ideas
that perhaps we might be able to implement locally.
These comments support the thinking of Brookfield (1981, 1984), Knowles
(1975), and others that self-direction does not necessarily mean that learning takes
place in isolation. In many cases, participants emphasized their identification as part of
a group of individuals with a common interest who could expand their knowledge by
sharing with each other.
Recognition of the Need for Ongoing Learning by Community Leaders
Community leaders strongly evidenced the need for and the practice of ongoing
learning in order to adequately fulfill their duties in both their work roles and their
community leadership roles, as documented in the quotes provided in an earlier
section of this paper. Their comments support Kouzes and Posner’s (1995) claim that
“effective leaders are constantly learning” (p. 323). The community leaders conducted
a large number and a wide variety of learning projects. Although the most prevalent
learning projects related to their jobs and the second most prevalent related to their
community leadership roles, it could be assumed that many of the learning projects
had a dual purpose of both work and community organization. For example, one
participant indicated that he was applying the learning from his workplace to his
leadership of the community organization.
The specific things that I have been learning are my new roles and
responsibilities that are required of me. We are part of an international
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
8
organization fostering good will in our local and global community. Right now I
am going through a lot of training that details how to motivate people to
accomplish goals [at work].
The demands of the participants’ jobs and their leadership positions appeared
to fuel both their need for continued learning and their recognition of their learning
capacity. One respondent said:
I’m finding that I’m needing to apply myself more in whatever; [for example,]
communicating more to larger groups than one-on-one. I mean both exist, but
the majority of my communication has been either one-on-one or in groups of
five or six, and this year that has flipped. And also, I’m having to readdress the
need [to be] much more sensitive to listening, not only to others, but also
myself. I’m continuing to school myself.
Another noted, “You realize that your learning capabilities are not limited when you
can put your mind to it and involve other people.” One final quote provides a good
summary of the community leaders’ recognition of their need to be continuous
learners: “I don’t think that you’re ever through learning. There is always something
else to learn.”
Implications For Practice
It is evident that the community leaders examined in this study are selfdirected
learners. They know how to take the initiative in diagnosing their own
learning needs and finding ways to meet those needs. They are lifelong learners who
value the importance of ongoing learning and have participated in a variety of learning
projects. They are aware of current technology and try to maintain their proficiency.
Therefore, outdated learning materials and traditional lecture-formatted educational
programs would not be acceptable to them. This sample was limited and may not
necessarily reflect the wider population of community leaders; however, it suggests
that training programs need to be developed and material presented in a manner that
recognizes that community leaders are likely to be self-directed learners. The issue of
time also needs to be addressed. Usually, community leaders are members of the
workplace in a professional or managerial position, belong to more than one
community organization, and participate in several different leadership training
programs. They have the enthusiasm to meet and learn with and from others and
exchange ideas. They are willing to share and are always looking for ways to do things
more effectively and efficiently. However, they do not have the time or desire to waste
on being spoon-fed information that they may have already mastered.
Trainers and developers of training programs need to take into consideration
that community leaders are likely to be self-directed learners and plan the educational
programs accordingly. The traditional training programs need to be reevaluated and
updated. Community leaders need to have some face-to-face contact with other
learners and be able to share ideas. The interviewees all spoke of the value of one-onSDL
and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
4
9
one and small group discussions as effective learning approaches. Planners need to
remember to provide space for collaborative learning and extensive sharing of lessons
that people have learned through their own efforts. The information needs to be useful
and applicable, cutting edge, and it needs to go beyond the boundaries of the local
community. All of the community leaders interviewed also reported learning projects
related to technology. This finding suggests that internet-based resources such as
discussion boards, desktop conferencing, web-based tutorials and listservs might be
excellent vehicles to assist community leaders.
Suggestions For Further Research
Further research on volunteer community leaders in other organizations and in
other cultures is needed. This study targeted a specific area of one state and included
only two types of organizations, the Rotary and community leadership organization
boards, limiting its generalizability. It would be assumed that other community
organization leaders are self-directed learners, but it would be interesting to identify
their learning projects and explore how these relate to their community leadership
roles.
Leadership is a key ingredient to strong communities. A convergence of
factors is making effective, insightful community leadership ever more essential in the
fabric of our society. Expanding responsibilities and challenges of community leaders
are being fueled by budget cuts and rapid changes in all aspects of our society. There
are many community needs not being met or inadequately being met. Development of
community leaders is a never-ending process, beginning with the identification of
potential leaders, drawing them into areas of involvement, and providing training
(Bloom, 1995). Continued research and support into the learning needs and methods
that will assist community leaders in effectively meeting the demands of their complex
roles is essential.
References
Benson, R. J. (2004). District 6990 Rotary International 2004-2005 Official Directory.
Available from Rotary District 6990, 269 Giralda Avenue, Suite 302, Coral
Gables, FL 33157.
Bloom, C. R. (1995, Spring). Developing community leadership. Planning
Commissioners Journal, 18. Retrieved from http://www.plannersweb.com/
articles/blo086.html
Boyce, L. A. (2004). Propensity for self-development of leadership attributes:
Understanding, predicting, and supporting leader self-development
performance. Dissertation Abstracts International 65(01), 468B.
Brasfield, L. (1984). Educational attainment and participation in self-directed
learning projects (Unpublished master’s thesis). University of Tennessee,
Knoxville, TN.
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
5
0
Brockett, R. G. (1985). Methodological and substantive issues in the measurement of
self-directed learning readiness. Adult Education Quarterly, 36(1), 15-24.
Brockett, R., & Hiemstra, R. (1991). Self-Direction in adult learning: Perspectives on
theory, research, and practice. London: Routledge.
Brookfield, S. (1981). Independent adult learning. Studies in Adult Education, 13, 15-
27.
Brookfield, S. (1984). Self-directed adult learning: A critical paradigm. Adult
Education Quarterly, 35, 59-71.
Bryan-Wunner, V. C. (1991). Verification of competencies needed by entry-level
recreation, park and leisure professional in Florida and the relationship of
competency perception to readiness for self-directed learning. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 52(05), 1891A.
Chuprina, L., & Durr, R. (2006). Self-directed learning readiness as a predictor of
cultural adaptability in expatriate managers. International Journal of Self-
Directed Learning, 3 (1), 13-23.
Clark, D. (1999). Chart 1: Leadership competency model or the pyramid of leadership.
Retrieved from http://www.nwlink.com/~donclark/hrd/case/ chart1.html
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Connolly, R. A. (2004). The correlation between self-directed learning behavior and
leadership effectiveness in a business environment. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 65 (07), 2454A.
Coolican, P. M. (1975). The learning styles of mothers of young children. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 35, 783A-784A.
Davis, C. A., Bailey, C., Nypaver, M., Rees, T., & Brockett, R. G. (2010). Learning
projects of graduate students: An update of Tough’s study. International
Journal of Self-Directed Learning, 7(1), 14-28.
Delahaye, B. L., & Choy, S. (2000). The Learning Preference Assessment (Self-
Directed Learning Readiness Scale). Review of the Learning Preference
Assessment (LPA). In J. Maltby, C. A. Lewis, & A. Hill (Eds.). The
handbook of psychological tests. Wales, UK: Edwin Mellen Press.
Delahaye, B.L., & Smith. H. (1995). The validity of the Learning Preference
Assessment. Adult Education Quarterly, 45(3), 159-173.
Durr, R. E. (1992). An examination of readiness and time spent in self-directed
learning and selected personnel variables at a large Midwestern electronics
development and manufacturing corporation. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 53, 18255.
Estrin, H. R. (1986). Life satisfaction and participation in learning activities among
widows. Dissertation Abstracts International, 46, 3852A.
Field, L. (1989). An investigation into the structure, validity and reliability of
Guglielmino’s Self-Directed Readiness Scale. Adult Education Quarterly,
39(4), 235-245.
Finestone, P. (1984). A construct validation of the Self-Directed Learning Readiness
Scale with labour education participants. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 46, 5A.
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
5
1
Glesne, C. (1999). Becoming qualitative researchers: An introduction. New York:
Longman.
Graeve, E. A. (1987). Patterns of self-directed learning of registered nurses.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 820.
Guglielmino, L. M. (1978). Development of the Self-Directed Learning Readiness
Scale. Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, 6467A.
Guglielmino, L. M. (1989). Guglielmino’s response to Field’s investigation. Adult
Education Quarterly, 39, 235-240.
Guglielmino, L. M. (1996). An examination of self-directed learning readiness and
selected demographic variables of top female executives. In H. B. Long &
Associates (Eds.), Current developments in self-directed learning (pp. 11-
22). Norman, OK: Public Managers Center, College of Education, University
of Oklahoma.
Guglielmino, L. M. (1997). Reliability and validity of the Self-Directed Learning
Readiness Scale and the Learning Preference Assessment. In H. B. Long &
Associates (Eds.), Expanding horizons in self-directed learning (pp. 209-
222). Norman, OK: Public Managers Center, College of Education,
University of Oklahoma.
Guglielmino, L. M., Asper, D., Findley, B., Lunceford, C., McVey, R. S., Payne, S.,
Penney, G., & Phares, L. (2005). Common barriers, interrupters and
restarters in the learning projects of self-directed adult learners. International
Journal of Self-Directed Learning, 2, 71-93.
Guglielmino, L. M. & Guglielmino, P. J. (1991). The Learning Preference Assessment
facilitator guide. King of Prussia, PA: Organization Design and
Development, Inc.
Guglielmino, L. M., & Guglielmino, P. J. (2008). Productivity in the workplace: The
role of self-directed learning and the implications for human resource
management. International Journal for Human Resoiurces Development and
Management, 8 (4), 293-305.
Guglielmino, P. J., & Guglielmino, L. M. (1983). An examination of the relationship
between self-directed learning readiness and job performance in a major
utility. Boca Raton, FL: Guglielmino & Associates.
Guglielmino, P. J., Guglielmino, L. M., & Long, H. B. (1987). Self-directed learning
and performance in the workplace. Higher Education, 16, 303-17.
Guglielmino, P. J., & Klatt, L. A. (1994). Self-directed learning readiness as a
characteristic of the entrepreneur. In H. B. Long & Associates (Eds.), New
ideas about self-directed learning (pp. 161-174). Norman, OK: Oklahoma
Research Center for Continuing Professional and Higher Education of the
University of Oklahoma.
Hassan, A. J. (1981). An investigation of the learning projects of adults of high and
low readiness for self-direction in learning. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 42, 3838A-3839A.
Hiemstra, R. (1976). The older adult’s learning projects. Educational Gerontology, 1,
331-341.
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
5
2
Imel, S. (1999). New views of adult learning. Trends and Issues (Alert No. 5).
Retrieved from http://www.calpro-online.org/eric/docs/tia00073.pdf
Kandarian, F. (2004). Executive learning related to high performance in two
companies. Dissertation Abstracts International, 65 (06), 2059A.
Knowles, M. (1975). Self-directed learning: A guide for learners and teachers. New
York: Association Press.
Kotter, J. P. (1996). Leading change. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.
Kotter, J. P. (1998, Fall). Winning at change. Leader to Leader, 10, 27-33.
Kouzes, J. M., & Posner, B. Z. (1995). The leadership challenge. San Francisco, CA:
Jossey-Bass.
Liddell, T. N. (2007). Learning and leading: Approaches to leadership selfdevelopment
of women executives of philanthropic organizations (Doctoral
dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest. (AAT 3269545)
Long, H. B. (1989). Some additional criticisms of Field’s investigation. Adult
Education Quarterly, 39, 240-243.
Long, H. B., & Agyekum, S. K. (1984). Teacher ratings in the validation of
Guglielmino’s Self-Directed Learning Readiness Scale. Higher Education, 13,
709-715.
McCune, S. K. (1988). A meta-analytic study of adult self direction in learning: A
review of the research from 1977 to 1987. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 49, 3237.
McCune, S. K. (1989). A statistical critique of Field’s investigation. Adult Education
Quarterly, 39, 243-246.
McCune, S. K., & Guglielmino, L. M. (1991). The validity generalization of
Guglielmino’s Self-Directed Learning Readiness Scale. In H. B. Long &
Associates (Eds.), Self-directed learning: Consensus and conflict (pp. 147-
154). Norman, OK: Oklahoma Research Center for Continuing Professional
and Higher Education, University of Oklahoma.
McCune, S. K., Guglielmino, L. M., & Garcia, G. (1990). Adult self-direction in
learning: A preliminary meta-analytic investigation of research using the
Self-Directed Learning Readiness Scale conflict (pp. 145-156). In H. B. Long
& Associates (Eds.), Advances in self-directed learning research. Norman,
OK: Oklahoma Research Center for Continuing Professional and Higher
Education.
Merriam, S. B., Caffarella, R.S., & Baumgartner, L. (2007). Learning in adulthood
(3rd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Muller, K. (2007). Emotional intelligence and self-directed learning readiness among
healthcare managers (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest. (AAT
3266421)
O’Connell, B. (2006, January). Keep the dream alive. Associations Now, 2(1), 7.
Oliveira, A. L., & Simões, A. (2006). Impact of socio-demographic and psychological
variables on the self-directedness of higher education students. International
Journal of Self-Directed Learning, 3 (1), 1-12.
Penland, P. R. (1978). Self-planned learning in America. Retrieved from ERIC
Database. (ED 154 987)
SDL and Community Leaders
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
5
3
Penland, P. R. (1979). Self-initiated learning. Adult Education Quarterly, 29, 170-179.
Peters, J. M., & Gordon, R. S. (1974). Adult learning projects: A study of adult
learning in urban and rural Tennessee. Knoxville: The University of
Tennessee, Knoxville. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 102
431)
Posner, F. G. (1990). A study of self-directed learning, perceived competence and
personal orientation among students in an open alternative high school.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 51, 813.
Ralston, P. A. (1981). Educational needs and activities of older adults: Their
relationship to senior center programs. Educational Gerontology, 7, 321-244.
Roberts, D. G. (1986). A study of the use of the Self-Directed Learning Readiness
Scale as related to selected organizational variables. Dissertation Abstracts
International, 47, 1218A.
Taylor, R. (2002). Expanding the understanding of self-directed learning: Community
action and innovative workplaces. Dissertation Abstracts International,
63,12.
Tough, A. (1971). The adult’s learning projects. Toronto, Canada: Ontario Institute
for Studies in Education.
Zsiga, P. L. (2007). Self-directed learning readiness, strategic thinking, and leader
effectiveness in directors of a national nonprofit organization. Retrieved from
ProQuest. (AAT 3269548)
Leatrice T. Phares (leephares@earthlink.net) has worked with Leadership Broward in
Florida and served as District Governor for Rotary District 6990 (Broward, Miami-
Dade, Monroe Counties & the Grand Bahama Island) in 2009-2010. She has also
served on the Board of Directors of the National Community Leadership Association
and has co-chaired the NCLA annual conference. She holds a doctoral degree in Adult
and Community Education, Educational Leadership, from Florida Atlantic University.
Lucy M. Guglielmino (lguglielmino@rocketmail.com) is senior professor of Adult and
Community Education in the Department of Educational Leadership and Research
Methodology at Florida Atlantic University. Her primary research area is self-directed
learning in a wide variety of contexts. She co-chairs the Board of Directors of the
International Society for Self-Directed Learning and has received the Malcolm
Knowles Award for significant lifelong contributions to the field of self-directed
learning.
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
54
SELF-EFFICACY AND THE LEARNER AUTONOMY PROFILE
Michael K. Ponton, Paul B. Carr, Christine T. Schuette, and
Gary J. Confessore
Abstract
In 2004, the Appraisal of Learner Autonomy (ALA) was created as a measure of
self-efficacy in autonomous learning. Since 2005, it has been offered in conjunction with
the Learner Autonomy Profile (LAP) and has been completed by over 2,000 subjects. The
purpose of this article is to present recent analyses to better articulate the usefulness of the
ALA within the context of the LAP and to discuss related implications to the study of
adult learning. The findings suggest that the ALA offers important explanatory utility in
understanding learner autonomy and predicting autonomous learning.
The Learner Autonomy Profile (LAP; licensed to Human Resource Development
Enterprises, HRDE) was initially developed as a battery of four instruments: the Inventory
of Learner Desire (ILD; cf. Meyer, 2001), the Inventory of Learner Resourcefulness (ILR;
cf. Carr, 1999), the Inventory of Learner Initiative (ILI; cf. Ponton, 1999), and the
Inventory of Learner Persistence (ILP; cf. Derrick, 2001). The purpose of the LAP is to
use these measures of the four conative factors of desire, resourcefulness, initiative, and
persistence (cf. Confessore, 1992) as a method of determining the extent to which an adult
tends to engage in agentic learning, which is a defining characteristic of autonomous
learning (Ponton, 1999, 2009). To this end, HRDE continued instrument refinement (Park
& Confessore, 2002) and currently engages in the coaching of those adults around the
world who are interested in increasing their learner autonomy.
In 2004, Ponton, Derrick, Carr, and Hall presented the Appraisal of Learner
Autonomy (ALA) as a measure of self-efficacy in autonomous learning. The construct of
self-efficacy has been supported empirically as an important mediator between motivation
and agency (Bandura, 1997); therefore, Ponton et al. (2004) argued that such a measure
was essential in furthering the understanding of learner autonomy. The 9-item final
version of the ALA (Ponton, Derrick, Hall, Rhea, & Carr, 2005) was argued as valid and
has been used as part of the LAP since its publication in 2005 (note that the ALA is
unlicensed and is available in its entirety in Ponton, Derrick, Hall, et al., 2005, for
research purposes). At this time, over 2,000 people have taken the ALA in conjunction
with the administration of the LAP by HRDE.
Ponton (1999) offered a definition of learner autonomy as “the characteristic of the
person who independently exhibits agency [i.e., intentional behavior] in learning
activities” (pp. 13-14). He argued that the construct of learner autonomy exists within the
cognitive/affective domains of the learner and that autonomous learning represents the
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
55
resultant conative manifestations (i.e., intentional action) of such latent autonomy. The
ILD was conceptualized as a preconative measure of the degree to which a person can act
intentionally in any domain of functioning (cf. Meyer, 2001, Inventory of Intentional
Behavior) whereas the ILR (Carr, 1999), ILI (Ponton, 1999), and ILP (Derrick, 2001)
were designed as conative measures within the domain of adult learning. Self-efficacy is a
belief of personal capability to engage successfully in a given performance (Bandura,
1997); therefore, the ALA—a measure of one’s belief in requisite ability to successfully
engage in autonomous learning—exists within the preconative domain similar to the ILD.
There has been potential ambiguity in the literature with respect to whether
conative constructs should be included in the learner autonomy or autonomous learning
domains (cf. Ponton, Derrick, Confessore, & Rhea, 2005; Ponton & Schuette, 2008).
Ponton, Derrick, Confessore, et al. (2005) stated the following:
It should be noted that showing resourcefulness, initiative, and persistence in one’s
learning is conceptually separable from what is measured…[by the ILR, ILI, and
ILP]. The ILR, ILI, and ILP are measures of intention to show resourcefulness,
initiative, and persistence. These instruments were developed in this manner
because it is not possible to know, a priori, whether or not study participants are
currently engaged in autonomous learning activities (cf. Ponton, 1999). Further
research is necessary to uncover the strength of the relationship between the
intention to engage in autonomous learning and the enactment of the behaviors of
autonomous learning, the latter being the exhibition of resourcefulness, initiative,
and persistence. (p. 86)
Thus, autonomous learning represents the actual manifestation of action related constructs
(e.g., resourcefulness, initiative, and persistence) and not merely an intention to manifest
such action. Using the conative measures of the ILR, ILI, and ILP to characterize
autonomous learning as was done in Ponton and Schuette (2008) is by proxy only as there
does not exist any way of knowing whether or not a randomly selected study participant is
currently engaged in an autonomous learning activity for a direct measure of autonomous
learning to be applied. In addition, as the present conative constructs are cognitively based
(e.g., anticipating the future benefits of learning as part of the ILR), such direct measures
cannot be limited to behavioral observations but rather must encompass a constellation of
measures associated with self-reported “action-related concepts” (Chapman & Skinner,
1985, p. 201) under the larger umbrella of action theory.
To test this conceptual differentiation between learner autonomy and autonomous
learning, Ponton and Schuette (2008) conducted a 2-factor confirmatory principal
component analysis (PCA) using ILD, ILR, ILI, and ILP data from a nonprobability
sample of 2,277 adults; insufficient ALA data precluded an inclusion of this measure in
the analysis at that time. The PCA results supported the hypothesized separation of learner
autonomy—represented by ILD measurements—and autonomous learning as represented
by proxy by the ILR, ILI, and ILP measurements. Based on these results, they proposed it
would be tenable to combine ILR, ILI, and ILP scores as a singular measure of
autonomous learning (i.e., a new variable) provided each measure were normalized by the
number of items in its respective scale (it could certainly be argued that normalization is
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
56
required at the subscale level as well; however, this argument has not been investigated to
date).
The continued use of the ALA in conjunction with the LAP has resulted in a data
set of sufficient size to continue this analysis; Comrey and Lee (as cited in Tabachnick &
Fidell, 2007, p. 613) state that a sample size of 1,000 is excellent for factor analysis. Note
that there is no necessary reason to continue to define autonomous learning via proxy
measure arguments in order to make comparisons to learner autonomy constructs;
theoretically, preconative and conative constructs should be separable as well. Thus, we
hypothesize that a 2-factor confirmatory PCA would support the separation of the ALA
and ILD vis-à-vis the ILR, ILI, and ILP based upon the conceptual separation of the
preconative and conative domains of learner autonomy. The purpose of this investigation
is to test this research hypothesis. Furthering our understanding of the relationship
between these measures will help us to continue to assess the appropriateness of making
causal arguments for facilitating autonomous learning using Fishbein and Ajzen’s (1975)
behavioral model that relates cognition, affection, and conation to intentional behavior.
Based on this continued understanding, future studies would require the use of structural
equation modeling to test directional relationships.
Method
Participants
The data from a nonprobability sample of 2,074 adults were analyzed. These data
represent a conglomeration of samples from numerous research studies in which both the
LAP and ALA were administered. The average age of the participants in this resultant
sample was 28.1 years (SD = 12.0). The majority were female (n = 1,496; P = 72.1%) and
the level of education was as follows: high school diploma/G.E.D., n = 1,205, P = 58.1%;
bachelor’s degree, n = 324, P = 15.6%; and graduate/professional degree, n = 518, P =
25.0% (note that 27 participants, P = 1.3%, did not respond to this field).
Results
Table 1 presents the intercorrelations between the five scales. All correlations are
significant at the .01 level (2-tailed), and the ILD moderately correlates with the ILR, ILI,
and ILP whereas these last three scales correlate highly with each other. The ALA
moderately correlates with the ILR, ILI, and ILP, and its correlation with the ILD is low.
(“Low,” “moderate,” and “high” correlation descriptions as per Hinkle, Wiersma, & Jurs,
1998, p. 120, for correlation ranges .30 to .50, .50 to .70, and .70 to .90, respectively.)
Internal consistency for each scale is reflected in the following Cronbach alpha
coefficients: ILD, .93; ILR, .96; ILI, .97; ILP, .97; and ALA, .89.
Inspection of histograms (not presented) suggests normality for all five measures
with each distribution having a slight negative skewness. Linearity is supported by the
product-moment correlations presented in Table 1; as PCA was performed as opposed to
factor analysis, multicollinearity is not a concern (no matrix inversion in PCA;
Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007).
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
57
Table 1. Intercorrelations Between Scales (N = 2074)
Scale 1 2 3 4 5
1. ILD – .573* .521* .549* .391*
2. ILR – .843* .854* .552*
3. ILI – .893* .592*
4. ILP – .577*
5. ALA –
*p < .01 (2-tailed)
Table 2 presents the factor loadings using exploratory, unrotated PCA performed
on the correlation matrix. Compared to factor analysis, PCA is the preferred method of
factor extraction for exploratory studies (Mertler & Vannatta, 2005, p. 250); thus, it was
used in this investigation for the purpose of data reduction where it is presumed that the
principal components are based upon the measured responses (DeCoster, 1998). The sole
purpose of performing this preliminary analysis was to determine if there was any initial
indication that the five scales were statistically unrelated, which would be in contrast to
their theoretical classification as salient aspects of learner autonomy. The Kaiser-Meyer-
Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy (MSA) and the Bartlett Test of Sphericity were
used to assess the suitability of the correlation matrix for factor analysis. For factor
analysis, the MSA index should be no less than 0.5 (Cureton & D’Agostino, 1983, p.
389). In addition, Bartlett’s χ2 should enable a rejection of the null hypothesis of no
difference between the correlation matrix and the identity matrix (i.e., common factors
cannot exist unless partial correlations between items exist; Norusis, 1988) although this
test is likely to be statistically significant for large sample sizes even with low correlations
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). The resultant MSA = .86 and Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity
approximate χ2(10, N = 2074) = 8,102.1, p < .001, suggest the sample was adequate for
PCA. Gorsuch (1983) states the first principal component represents the best condensation
of a group of variables; thus, because the ILD, ILR, ILI, ILP, and ALA are linked to a
related theoretical construct (i.e., learner autonomy), it should be no surprise that the
loadings are high (Gorsuch asserts a minimum salient loading to be 0.3, p. 210, which is
consistent with Tabachnick & Fidell’s suggestion to only interpret variables with loadings
of 0.32 or greater, p. 649) in the first component. Note that the highest loadings—all
greater than 0.9—are for the ILR, ILI, and ILP scales.
Table 2. Exploratory Principal Component Analysis: All Scales
Scale Loading
ILD .698
ILR .920
ILI .928
ILP .933
ALA .722
Note. Only one component extracted explaining 71.7% of the total variance.
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
58
The intercorrelation and PCA results suggest that the hypothesized grouping of
ILR, ILI, and ILP scales versus a grouping of ILD and ALA may be testable using linear
methods. Thus, a confirmatory PCA was performed on the correlation matrix for a twofactor
solution using Oblimin rotation with Kaiser normalization (two factors were chosen
to correspond to the preconative and conative constructs of learner autonomy). Note that
oblique rotation was chosen because it would be reasonable to expect that preconative and
conative aspects of learner autonomy would correlate—conation results from beliefs as
per Fishbein and Ajzen (1975)—thus making oblique rotation tenable. The resultant
correlation between the two components is 0.44 (see Table 3), which is greater than the
0.32 minimum recommended by Tabachnick and Fidell (2007, p. 646) as justifying
oblique rotation. As is evident in Table 3, the loadings for the ILR, ILI, ILP, and ALA are
highest for the first component whereas the ILD loading is highest for the second
component; cross loadings do not suggest a more complex interpretation than this
separation. This is in contrast to the hypothesized 2-factor solution separating preconation
as represented by the ILD and ALA versus conation as represented by the ILR, ILI, and
ILP.
Table 3. Pattern Matrix for Confirmatory 2-Factor PCA: All Scales
Scale Loading
Component 1 Component 2
ILD .097 .902
ILR .747 .302
ILI .840 .182
ILP .807 .236
ALA .922 -.242
Note. Extraction sums of squared loadings: (a) for Component 1, 3.587 (71.7% of the total
variance); for Component 2, .617 (12.3% of the total variance). Rotation (Oblimin with
Kaiser normalization) sums of squared loadings: (a) for Component 1, 3.352; for
Component 2, 1.955. Correlation between Components 1 and 2: r = .44.
Because the ILR, ILI, ILP, and ALA constituted the first principal component, a
hierarchical regression analysis was performed to determine the predictive utility of the
ALA on conation. Note that a new variable conative learner autonomy was created by
summing ILR, ILI, and ILP scores where each is normalized by its respective number of
items (i.e., 53, 44, and 34, respectively; cf. Ponton & Schuette, 2008). The ALA was
chosen as the baseline model (i.e., Step 1a; see Table 4), and because of the statistically
significant correlation between the ILD and the other four scales, the ILD was added to
the ALA in Step 2. Both Step 1a and Step 2 models are significant at the .001 level; F(1,
2072) = 1179.1 and F(2, 2071) = 1025.2, respectively. The change in R2 from Step 1a to 2
(i.e., .135) is also significant at the .001 level.
If the ILD were chosen as the independent variable for conative learner autonomy in a
second baseline model (i.e., Step 1b; see Table 4), the model is also significant, F(1, 2072)
= 1016.2, p < .001, with R2 = .329 versus .363 when using the ALA as the independent
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
59
variable. As would be expected, the change in R2 by adding the ALA as a second
independent variable to this new baseline model (i.e., .169) is also significant at the .001
level. Thus, the ALA is a slightly stronger predictor for conative learner autonomy when
compared to the ILD due to an increase of 3.4% (i.e., .363 – .329) in explained variance.
Table 4. Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Conative
Learner Autonomy (ILRnorm + ILInorm + ILPnorm)
Variable B SE B β
Step 1a
ALA .015 .000 .602**
Step 1b
ILD .057 .002 .574**
Step 2
ALA .011 .000 .446**
ILD .040 .002 .399**
Note. R2 = .363 for Step 1a; R2 = .329 for Step 1b; R2 = .498 for Step 2 (p < .001 for
change from either Step 1a or 1b).
**p < .001.
Focusing on the ILR, ILI, ILP, and ALA and following the hypothesized
separation of preconation (i.e., ALA) and conation (i.e., ILR, ILI, and ILP), a
confirmatory PCA was performed on the correlation matrix for a two-factor solution using
Oblimin rotation with Kaiser normalization for the ILD, ILR, ILI, and ALA. MSA = .83
and Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity approximate χ2(6, N = 2074) = 7,224.0, p < .001; thus,
the sample was deemed adequate for PCA using this reduced variable set. In addition, the
correlation between components is 0.60 (see Table 5) thereby supporting oblique rotation.
As is evident in Table 5, the loadings for the ILR, ILI, and ILP are highest for the first
component whereas the ALA loading is highest for the second component; cross loadings
do not suggest a more complex interpretation than this separation.
Table 5. Pattern Matrix for Confirmatory 2-Factor PCA:
ILD Scale Excluded
Scale Loading
Component 1 Component 2
ILR .962 -.031
ILI .934 .036
ILP .960 .001
ALA .001 .999
Note. Extraction sums of squared loadings: (a) for Component 1, 3.181 (79.5% of the total
variance); for Component 2, .548 (13.7% of the total variance). Rotation (Oblimin with Kaiser
normalization) sums of squared loadings: (a) for Component 1, 3.088; for Component 2, 1.986.
Correlation between Components 1 and 2: r = .60.
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
60
Discussion
The research hypothesis is not supported by the findings; that is, the expected
separation of the ILD and ALA (within the preconative domain of learner autonomy)
versus the ILR, ILI, and ILP (within the conative domain of learner autonomy) is not
tenable (see Table 3). The factor loadings associated with the ILR, ILI, ILP, and ALA on
the first principal component suggest a common latent construct among these measures.
Meyer (2001) created an instrument that assesses the degree to which an adult can
act intentionally based upon three constituent subscales: basic freedoms, managing power,
and acquired skills. From her original instrument titled the Inventory of Intentional
Behavior, the ILD evolved; however, the ILD does not actually represent a measure
within the context of learning but rather represents a measure of theoretical importance to
any agentic action of which autonomous learning is but one example. As Park and
Confessore (2002) stated, “[Meyer’s] work on desire to learn has been treated as an effort
to understand the precursors to the development of intentions related to learning” (p. 289).
In contrast to the ILD, the ILR, ILI, ILP, and ALA are contextualized to learning.
Carr’s (1999) ILR assesses the degree to which an adult (a) anticipates the future benefits
of learning, (b) prioritizes learning over nonlearning activities, (c) chooses to engage in
learning versus nonlearning activities, and (d) solves problems that impede desired
learning. Ponton’s (1999) ILI assesses the following behavioral intentions in an adult
learner as manifest with respect to a learning activity: goal-directedness, action
orientation, persistence in overcoming obstacles, active approach to problem solving, and
self-startedness. Derrick’s (2001) ILP measures the sustained maintenance of the
following behaviors in learning: volition, self-regulation, and goal-directedness. Finally,
the ALA (Ponton, Derrick, Hall, et al., 2005) measures the perceived capability of an adult
to engage in autonomous learning in the face of impediments to personal agency.
In the PCA model, “the principal components are based on the measured
responses” (DeCoster, 1998, p. 3); thus, our interpretation of the results presented in Table
3 is that the first principal component is associated with learner autonomy based on beliefs
of efficacy and intentions to exhibit resourcefulness, initiative, and persistence within the
context of learning. The ILR, ILI, ILP, and ALA are all contextualized to adult learning
and have been argued as together supporting autonomous learning; however, the ILD is
not contextualized to learning. Therefore, the PCA results may have separated the five
variables along the dimension of learning, which appears theoretically possible. When this
dimension is controlled (i.e., when the ILD is removed from the PCA; see Table 5), factor
loadings again support the theoretical separation of preconative learner autonomy (related
to the ALA) and conative learner autonomy (related to the ILR, ILI, and ILP).
The present results suggest that the reason asserted by Ponton and Schuette (2008)
for the separation of the ILD vis-à-vis the ILR, ILI, and ILP may not be the relationship
between preconation and conation but rather is a result of the varied contextualization to
learning; however, this could not have been assessed in 2008 without the ALA data.
Controlling for learning contextualization results in a component structure that still
supports the conclusion of Ponton and Schuette (2008) regarding the appropriateness for
summing normalized ILR, ILI, and ILP scores into a new variable existing within the
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
61
conative domain of learner autonomy versus the preconative domain of learner autonomy
as represented in part by the ALA.
The separate, predictive utility of either the ALA or the ILD on a new variable
conative learner autonomy is statistically significant at the .001 level and qualitatively
similar: R2 = .363 for the ALA versus .329 for the ILD. In addition, using both as
independent variables, the total variance explained in conative learner autonomy is 49.8%
(see Table 4), which compares reasonably to the 59.7% previously reported by Ponton,
Derrick, Confessore, et al. (2005) in their preliminary study of 82 adults using the same
independent variables but rather a summation of nonnormalized ILR, ILI, and ILP scores
for a reduced variable. Note that the addition of either the ALA or the ILD to the model
results in a statistically significant increase in R2 at the .001 level; thus, the model is more
fully specified when both scales are included. The low correlation between the ILD and
ALA (see Table 1) suggests that each accounts for separate variance in conative learner
autonomy although the 49.8% of variance explained suggests that there are still more
preconative measures (e.g., motivation, personal responsibility) required to fully specify a
prediction model.
The degree to which a person believes him or herself generally capable of acting
agentively, which is assessed by the ILD, will manifest itself in the intentional activities,
or lack thereof, of the agent. The statistical findings associated with the ILD, ILR, ILI, and
ILP are consistent in numerous studies over several years in that the ILD has always
exhibited a statistically significant and moderate to high correlation with the other three
measures either separately or in summation; thus, the degree of extant agency is well
established as being related to the degree to which an adult intends to engage in
autonomous learning. We find it interesting, however, that the ALA does exhibit some
interesting statistical properties when compared to the ILD: (a) it loads with the ILR, ILI,
and ILP along the proposed dimension of learner autonomy; (b) it loads separately from
the ILR, ILI, and ILP when the dimension of learning is controlled along the argued
dimensions of preconation versus conation; and (c) it accounts for more variance (albeit
slightly) with respect to the reduced variable conative learner autonomy. However, the
regression model associated with the criterion variable conative learner autonomy is more
fully specified when both the ILD and ALA are included as independent variables.
Thus, we assert that the ALA offers some important explanatory utility in
understanding learner autonomy and predicting autonomous learning. Specifically, in
support of HRDE’s coaching interests, the ALA should be offered as part of the LAP and
inform resultant interventions that promote learner autonomy using the sources of efficacy
information outlined by Bandura (1997): mastery experiences, verbal persuasion,
vicarious experiences, and interpretations of physiological/emotive arousals. Generally, as
we continue to further our understanding of adult learning, the ALA should be used in
conjunction with other studies to continue to define and inform the causal role of selfefficacy
in agentic learning.
References
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: W. H.
Freeman and Company.
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
62
Carr, P. B. (1999). The measurement of resourcefulness intentions in the adult
autonomous learner. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 3849.
Chapman, M., & Skinner, E. A. (1985). Action in development–Development in action.
In M. Frese & J. Sabini (Eds.), Goal-directed behavior: The concept of action in
psychology (pp. 200-213). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Confessore, G. J. (1992). An introduction to the study of self-directed learning. In G. J.
Confessore & S. J. Confessore (Eds.), Guideposts to self-directed learning: Expert
commentary on essential concepts (pp. 1-6). King of Prussia, PA: Organization Design
and Development.
Cureton, E. E., & D’Agostino, R. B. (1983). Factor analysis: An applied approach.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
DeCoster, J. (1998). Overview of factor analysis. Retrieved from http://www.stathelp.
com/factor.pdf
Derrick, M. G. (2001). The measurement of an adult’s intention to exhibit persistence in
autonomous learning. Dissertation Abstracts International, 62, 2533.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction
to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). Factor analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hinkle, D. E., Wiersma, W., & Jurs, S. G. (1998). Applied statistics for the behavioral
sciences (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Mertler, C. A., & Vannatta, R. A. (2005). Advanced and multivariate statistical methods:
Practical application and interpretation (3rd ed.). Glendale, CA: Pyrczak.
Meyer, D. T. (2001). The measurement of intentional behavior as a prerequisite to
autonomous learning. Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 4697.
Norusis, M. J. (1988). SPSS-X advanced statistics guide (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: SPSS.
Park, E., & Confessore, G. J. (2002). Development of new instrumentation: Validation of
the Learner Autonomy Profile Beta version. In H. B. Long & Associates (Eds.),
Twenty-first century advances in self-directed learning (pp. 289-306). Schaumburg,
IL: Motorola University Press.
Ponton, M. K. (1999). The measurement of an adult’s intention to exhibit personal
initiative in autonomous learning. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 3933.
Ponton, M. K. (2009). An agentic perspective contrasting autonomous learning with selfdirected
learning. In M. G. Derrick & M. K. Ponton (Eds.), Emerging directions in
self-directed learning (pp. 65-76). Chicago, IL: Discovery Association Publishing
House.
Ponton, M. K., Derrick, M. G., Carr, P. B., & Hall, J. M. (2004, February). The
relationship between self-efficacy and autonomous learning. Paper presented at the
18th International Self-Directed Learning Symposium, Cocoa Beach, FL.
Ponton, M. K., Derrick, M. G., Confessore, G. J., & Rhea, N. E. (2005). The role of selfefficacy
in autonomous learning. International Journal of Self-Directed Learning,
2(2), 81-90. Retrieved from http://www.sdlglobal.com
Ponton, M. K., Derrick, M. G., Hall, J. M., Rhea, N. E., & Carr, P. B. (2005). The
relationship between self-efficacy and autonomous learning: The development of new
instrumentation. International Journal of Self-Directed Learning, 2(1), 50-61.
Retrieved from http://www.sdlglobal.com
Self-Efficacy and the Learner Autonomy Profile
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
63
Ponton, M. K., & Schuette, C. T. (2008). The Learner Autonomy Profile: A discussion of
scale combination to measure autonomous learning. International Journal of Self-
Directed Learning, 5(1), 55-60. Retrieved from http://www.sdlglobal.com
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston,
MA: Pearson Education.
Michael Ponton (michpon@regent.edu) is Professor of Education at Regent University.
His research interest is in the development of a better understanding of the role of human
agency in self-directed learning.
Paul Carr (paulca2@regent.edu) is Professor of Global Leadership and Entrepreneurship
at Regent University. His research interests are in resourcefulness in learning, adult
learning, and autonomous learning.
Christine Schuette (cschuette@regent.edu) is Assistant Professor of Education at Regent
University. Her research interests are in social psychology with particular emphasis in
social and moral development.
Gary Confessore (gjcon@gwu.edu) is Professor Emeritus of Higher Education
Administration at The George Washington University Graduate School of Education and
Human Development.
Author Note
The authors would like to acknowledge the assistance provided by EunMi Park in
managing the data analyzed for this investigation.
The data analyzed in this paper were acquired via various research studies (under
the license held by Human Resource Development Enterprises) and not for the sole
purpose of this investigation.
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
64
FOSTERING SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING IN AN HONORS
CLASSROOM THROUGH UNCONVENTIONAL METHODS AND
ASSESSMENT
Nancy D. McDonald and Idell McLaughlin
Abstract
Instructors familiar with the attributes of self-directed learning (SDL) sense their
resonance with the Honors course objectives set forth by the National Collegiate Honors
Council (2008). Consequently, it seems Honors classrooms present ideal opportunities for
exploring SDL, not only in instructional approaches, but also in evaluation and
assessment. Assessment can be integrated so it becomes not a terminal evaluation of a
specific learning activity but an open-ended element in a continuous progression of
learning. This article is an exploration of how such an assessment approach has been
implemented in Honors classes at Palm Beach State College, how that implementation
encourages SDL, and the corresponding observable results.
The National Collegiate Honors Council (2008), in a statement on Honors Course
design outlining course objectives, noted the following: “The key to a successful Honors
program is not the intelligence of the student or the subject matter of the course, but the
attitude and approach of the instructor” (2008, p. 1). In order to support and guide
instructors through a process of designing an Honors course, the Council delineated five
objectives that, either in this form or some variation, should be included in most Honors
courses:
1. To help students develop effective written communication skills (including
the ability to make effective use of the information and ideas they learn);
2. To help students develop effective oral communication skills (while
recognizing that not all students are comfortable talking a lot in class);
3. To help students develop their ability to analyze and synthesize a broad
range of material;
4. To help students understand how scholars think about problems, formulate
hypotheses, research those problems, and draw conclusions about them;
and to help students understand how creative artists approach the creative
process and produce an original work;
5. To help students become more independent and critical thinkers,
demonstrating the ability to use knowledge and logic when discussing an
issue or an idea, while considering the consequences of their ideas, for
themselves, for others, and for society. (p. 1)
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
65
As delineated and academically supportive as these objectives are, there is, as with
all learning objectives, an understood challenge, that of assessment. Assessment is always
the other side of objectives, the cart behind the horse. How one connects the two informs
the manner in which material and media of the instruction are configured. The challenge
can be broken down into three considerations:
1. How can the instructor assess if students have been successful in meeting
the Honors course objectives?
2. How can the instructor guarantee that assessments offer equal opportunities
for all students?
3. How can the instructor integrate assessments into a continuous learning
cycle?
These considerations prod an instructor to think outside the usual approaches to
assessment, opening the door to a new consideration for enhancing student success and
their ability to become independent and critical thinkers—the field of self-directed
learning. To instructors aware of the strengths of self-directed learning (SDL), the
alignment of Honors course objectives and self-directed learner attributes is obvious.
Those attributes, as delineated by Guglielmino (1978), include the following:
§ exhibits initiative, independence, and persistence in learning;
§ accepts responsibility for [personal] learning and views problems as
challenges, not obstacles;
§ is capable of self-discipline and has a high degree of curiosity;
§ has a strong desire to learn or change and is self-confident;
§ is able to use basic study skills, organize [personal] time and set an appropriate
pace for learning, and to develop a plan for completing work; and,
§ enjoys learning and has a tendency to be goal-oriented. (p. 73)
These suggest that the Honors classroom presents an exciting opportunity for
exploring self-directed learning, not only in instruction, but also in evaluation and
assessment. As stated in Mok (2010), assessment, as it is usually thought of, is in need of
change. In particular, she believes there are three principles that should be the basis for
designing assessments: “Namely, that assessments should be designed as learning task[s];
that assessment should engage students in the evaluation of [their own and their peers’]
performance; and that feedback should be used as feedforward in order to support current
and future learning” (p. 14). In order to accomplish this redesign, it is necessary to
completely rethink the concept and integrate assessment in such a way that it becomes
part of a continuous learning progression, not a terminal evaluation of a specific learning
activity or module.
This paper is an exploration of how a continuous learning cycle approach to
assessment has been implemented in several Honors College classes at Palm Beach State
College. It also describes how such an implementation effort encourages SDL and what
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
66
results have been observable from the process. The use of strategies to promote selfdirection
in learning in the areas of narrative learning, somatic learning, and imagitive
learning will be examined. These three areas, with their emphasis on independence,
creativity, analysis, goal-setting, organization, and time management and their
encouragement of open-ended questions and projects, seem especially well-suited for
fostering self-direction in learning.
Narrative Approaches
Most class assignments can be a means of assessment as well as an opportunity for
extrapolated learning experiences. Unfortunately, some assignments, by nature or
psychological connections, can have a detrimental effect on learning. One assignment
almost universally dreaded by students begins with these words: “Write about . . . .”
Many students, even those in Honors, fear writing. Whether or not the fear is based in
reality, it can have a paralytic effect that interferes with students’ abilities to express
themselves with clarity and intelligence. Students who are ordinarily articulate in
classroom discussions may experience a terrible frustration when confronted with a blank
piece of paper or a newly opened computer document. That frustration dams ideas,
connections, and creativity. Journal writing provides a simple and viable way to defuse
that initial response, to make writing a natural expression medium as normal and
acceptable as cell phone usage.
Journaling has been universally extolled as a gateway to self-knowledge (Boud,
2001; Dirkx, 2001; English & Gillen, 2001; Hiemstra, 2001; Jarvis, 2001, Karpiak, 2002;
Mezirow & Associates, 1990). Boud (2001) observes that it provides an opportunity to
engage students in reflective practice—practice that encourages both self-directed learning
and transformative learning. In Journal Writing as an Adult Learning Tool, Kerka (2002a)
underscored the connection between writing, reflection, and learning. She also addressed
approaches to evaluating journals. Suggested methodologies, including coding, were
considered, but the initial question she posed remained: What is more important: process
or product? Until that is decided, it is almost impossible to make decisions about
evaluation. One obvious answer is to transcend the usual division and make product and
process identical.
The very act of journaling requires reflection at some level, creating opportunities
for transformative learning. Mezirow and Associates (2000) outlined three elements of
transformative learning: experience, critical reflection, and development. They divided
reflection into three subdivisions: content, process, and premise. It is essential that
journaling activity provide an opportunity for multi-layered reflection as a natural function
of the activity; that is, the activity should be structured so that what is produced flows as
freely as possible—in much the same way as a pre-teen’s diary. Tailoring an assignment
to fit this parameter is manageable through simplifying requirements. Hiemstra (2001)
provided a breakdown of journal types, usages, advantages and limitations. It is possible
to find an initial template among these choices and then alter configuration to fit purpose.
In this application, journal writing on its simplest level was introduced in a firstsemester
composition course. The instructions were straightforward and encouragingly
open-ended. The student was responsible for obtaining a bound journal and creating two,
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
67
100-word, hand-written entries weekly. Subject matter was neither assigned nor limited—
with one exception: if a student was engaged in felonious activities, these were off limits.
Otherwise, students were guaranteed confidentiality, with no one but the instructor
reading the journals. Vocabulary, spelling, and grammar were not checked. The only
thing not allowed was vulgar language. At the end of eight weeks (midterm), the student
was simply responsible for 1600 words. The instructor also informed the students that
there would be no feedback on the journals, no comments written in the journals.
The journal’s primary objective, in this application, was to dissipate students’
writing anxiety; the secondary objective was to encourage critical reflection. Although
critical reflection was not formally evaluated, it should be noted that while many students
began with fairly superficial entries, by the end of the activity almost every student was
writing longer, more thoughtful pieces. In fact, a number of the journals far exceeded the
required word count, and many students asked if they could continue the project and
receive extra credit. The answer was yes.
Despite substantial research demonstrating that feedback is “one of the most
powerful factors influencing leaning and achievement” (Mok, 2010, p. 17), feedback, in
this case, it was likely to have proven counterproductive. It would have focused the
students’ attention on the fact of writing and having that writing evaluated instead of
encouraging the student to just relax and do it.
The use of narrative writing of a more sophisticated nature, however, can be
demonstrated by the following example from Honors World Literature before the
Renaissance. In this course there are a number of readings conducive to reflective
writing; for example, is the Confessions of St. Augustine is especially appropriate. In
general, autobiographical writing has a deeply spiritual component (Dirkx, 2001;
Dominice, 2000; Foehr & Schiller, 1997; Hiemstra, 2001; Karpiak, 2002; Tisdell, 2003).
Its content reflects much more than a narrative, containing art, theory, and philosophy
(Karpiak, 2005), and opens the author to the possibility of a transformative learning
experience. Consequently, the more self-directed the writing experience is, the more likely
transformative learning will take place.
In the Honors class, the Confessions was studied at mid-semester. The students
read book selections including his learning to speak, the pear tree incident, time in
Carthage, conversion, and spiritual evaluation of his mother. In-depth discussions were
conducted in which students considered Augustine’s motivation, audience, methodology,
selection of material, and life experiences that impacted who he was and what he became.
At the assignment’s completion, students were informed that the final examination would
be an individual exercise. Each student was to write a 20-page minimum Confessions ala
St. Augustine. In it, students were to submit their lives to the same scrutiny that Augustine
employed. Although the audience did not have to be God, students were urged to pick
someone as audience in order to give the work consistency and focus. Life-defining
incidents were to be explored, both positive and negative, in order to gain insight into
motivations and choices.
Again, confidentiality and trust were essential. Unless a deep trust was built
between instructor and students during the first half of the course, this assignment could
be useless. The students would be guarded in what they said and refrain from deeply
reflective writing. To further this trust, the instructor of this course, during the discussion
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
68
of the Confessions, recounted incidents in her own life that were benchmarks. Some of the
occurrences were either neutral or positive events, but two of the episodes were negative,
and she illustrated how these had given rise to valuable personal insights into herself and
her life. By sharing personal events, the instructor further augmented the trust already
established. Personal reassurance during the rest of the term was also important. The
instructor periodically asked about progress, concerns, and experiences. Any initial
trepidation eased as time passed and familiarity with the assignment was established. It
was also essential that students be reminded this was not a paper designed as an
exploration of writing errors. It was, rather, a paper that investigated who the student had
become. It was a unique opportunity to receive course credit for taking the time to
consider who they were—something for which their fast-paced lives left them little time.
This assignment/assessment was not introduced without adequate SDL preparation
and encouragement throughout the entire term. In addition to in-depth consideration of the
Confessions prior to making the assignment, students were encouraged throughout class to
develop SDL attributes. This was done through structuring open-ended classroom
discussions, assigning short reflective papers, and supporting other activities that
promoted individual investigation and exploration.
When first examining the Confessions assignment, there might be a tendency to
view it as an interesting assignment but not as an assessment; however, in the truest sense
of the word, it was not only an assessment but also an exercise in self-directed learning. It
opened student assessment into self-reflection, potential transformation, and lifelong
learning and self-development. Students had been invited to experience these processes
from the beginning of class, and the Confessions assignment/assessment was a
culmination of that learning. It differed from most assessments in one respect only:
feedback. Because of the powerful, personal material elicited and the remarkable insights
recorded in the individual pieces, it would have been not only inappropriate but
counterproductive to comment on the work. The Confessions were private—the instructor
was simply allowed to read them. The contract for the grade was fulfilled in the writing.
This final class assessment has been in use each Fall term for the past eight years.
The resultant works, without exception, have been moving testaments to triumph and
failure; sadness and joy; struggle and loss; and, most of all, to survival and determination,
the brave beauty that is the best part of humanity.
Somatic Approaches
Another approach to SDL can be made through somatic learning. Somatic or
embodied learning, as defined by Merriam, Caffarella, and Baumgartner (2007),
is most often linked to experiential learning in the sense that we learn in the
experience. Somatic knowing, as is also true of spiritual and narrative knowing, is
connected to adult learning through meaning-making. Attending to these
noncognitive dimensions of knowing can bring greater understanding to our lives;
they enable us to make meaning of our everyday experiences. Learning in the
experience is immediate, physical, emotional. (p. 192)
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
69
Kerka (2002b) envisioned somatic learning as a holistic approach that seeks to
correct the western tendency to separate body and mind. Somatic learning integrates the
body—senses, perception, and movement—into the learning experience. Also pertinent is
Lawrence’s (2005) exploration of the implications of multiple intelligences and
indigenous knowledge when leading students to a deeper understanding of self, world, and
his discussion of the implementation of art as a way of accessing and uncovering hidden
knowledge in students.
In two Honors classes, Honors World Literature before the Renaissance and
Honors English Literature before 1800, somatic learning was drawn upon in a unique
manner. The embodiment that was required involved a minimum of actual physical
movement but a maximum of intellectual, spiritual, and emotional identification. It was
an approach leaning heavily on internal transformation, which, in turn, informed physical
presence; and, the mutual change was initiated by artistic experience. In these class
assignments, the insights provided both Kerka and Lawrence were important. For Honors
English Literature before 1800, students were asked to rewrite Chaucer’s Canterbury
Tales, setting it in a different time and peopling it with totally different pilgrims. For this
project, the class was divided into groups of three. After self-selecting membership
groups, students were informed of the assignment’s general outline but assured that
creative construction of the work would be left completely to them. Once students
understood the assignment and requirements, they met as a class and decided in which era
to set the new pilgrimage. The consensus was to place the work in the present. Although
the instructor provided a site with web links to Canterbury, both town and cathedral, bus
schedules, train schedules, and airlines, the students were left to figure out where they
would begin their pilgrimage and how they would make it last long enough for everyone
to tell tales.
Once basic structure was approved, students broke into small groups and decided
what members would be in terms of pilgrim identities. For example, one group decided
there would be a psychiatrist, the psychiatrist’s patient, and the patient’s hallucination.
Another group chose to come from a Latin American country and be private school
students in a religious club. The young woman chose to be a true member of the club, a
member with gently pious bearing. One young man chose as his character a holier-thanthou,
nerdy prig. The third student’s character was a club member only because he liked
the girl and was going on the pilgrimage to get a date. Another group was an
environmentalist, an industrialist, and a Congresswoman. After all roles were decided, the
research and writing began. Each student was charged with contributing three separate
pieces: a section on the chosen character to the General Prologue, a complete personal
prologue that preceded the character’s tale, and, of course, a tale appropriate to the
character. Web links to several Tale databases were also provided but most students chose
to make up their own tales. To make the experience more authentic, students were
encouraged to make the work rhyme.
The completed work, entitled The Canterbury Project, was presented on final
examination day. Students acted out their appropriate parts. It was astounding—moving,
funny, exhilarating and surprising. The overall experience for students and instructor,
alike, was gratifying and transformative. It was a fine example of self-directed learning
rooted in somatic learning supported by an artistic creation.
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
70
Following the same somatic philosophies outlined for the Canterbury assignment
and also incorporating Brockman’s (2001) position that a somatic epistemology can
provide a moral foundation to consider cultural goods and cultural evils, the Honors
World Literature before the Renaissance class members each were given the task of
analyzing and rewriting a canto from Dante’s Divine Comedy. The instructions were
simple: Each student was to pick a canto, become Dante, chose a suitable guide, re-people
the circle or terrace with appropriate sinners, repentants, or saints from the modern era,
add explanatory footnotes if necessary, make it 33 lines long, and make it rhyme terza
rima style. Although the whole could not really be assembled into a coherent piece since
the individual interpretations of Dante and his guide precluded that, the class enjoyed
hearing each individual’s canto read. And, strangely enough, although unplanned, a sort of
“class Dantean journey” emerged.
Imagitive Approaches
Embracing multiple prototypes, Honors instructors encourage experimentation
with unconventional techniques to assist the learning process. When teaching literature,
instructors introduce Honors students to a variety of genres. The “Dantean journey”
complements another creative assignment introduced as a learning cycle continuation, one
in which students explored, experimented, and employed their knowledge while creating
new and dynamic art works. Garrison (1992) notes that meaningful learning occurs when
learners assume shared responsibility for their educational process. Instructors can use
student-generated visuals to motivate students to become actively involved in the learning
process. This process can be instrumental in promoting SDL, modeling it, creating a
positive environment, introducing dramatic experiences and matching experiences to
student demands (Gibbons, 2008). The magnitude of the instructor’s importance in
fostering students’ self-direction in learning cannot be overstated. Gibbons suggests that
when students’ self-directed learning efforts bring success, that success is a powerful
motivator for continued learning. The imagitive approach integrates SDL and motivation
that leads to success. Defined, the imagitive approach is a creative process that encourages
students to incorporate visual images as an expression of their literal understanding of
literature, especially poetry.
Predictably, poetry’s compressed language poses difficulties for many Honors
students who find some poems too abstract and complex. As a result, countless students
struggle to understand themes, patterns, key concepts, metaphors, and imagery. The
incorporation of student-generated visuals can minimize students’ perplexity. Visuals
encourage students to find their inner voices and be creative while using their critical
thinking skills. Giving students freedom to create an image for interpreting poetry is an
innovative way to engage and assess Honors students, while at the same time promoting
self-direction in learning. The assignment/assessment begins a transformative process that
engages the class and affects the learning outcomes, facilitating understanding and
appreciation.
Instructors may question whether visuals can be utilized effectively beyond
illustrative purposes. First, note that the visuals used for this assignment were not
downloaded Internet images, but original visuals that students constructed and
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
71
incorporated in the literary discussion. Research indicates that images integrated in the
classroom deepen understanding and engage students through interpretation,
argumentation and analysis (Little & Felten, 2010), producing results for students and
teachers (Stokes, 2007).
When integrating visuals into teaching poetry, it is important not to restrict
students to a single visual medium. Students should be encouraged to utilize a variety of
illustrations, including posters, paintings, drawings, power points, sculptures, personal
photos, or objects. By creating their own visuals, students can make meaning out of
poetry by combining their knowledge and experience with imagery, motifs and symbols
found within a specific poem. The process begins by allowing students to select a poem.
Students are strongly discouraged from going on-line and reading literary commentaries
from sources such as Sparknotes.com. Instead, they are advised to read biographical
sketches and to review the social, political, and cultural climate that coincides with the
poem’s time era. The literary anthology selected for the course in this example provided
excellent biographical sketches as well as pertinent historical data, but as self-directed
learners, students were expected to rely also on their on their own research, creativity, and
intuition. As students read to understand the poem’s interior meaning, they mentally
recorded the mood, feeling or thoughts the poem conjured within. The objective was to
create a concrete visual image that emphasized a single literary feature or multiple
elements.
The student was then expected to unify the visual image with the poetry analysis.
As a class activity, students presented their poems and displayed their visuals. Each
presenter engaged the class by a reading derived from the poem. Afterwards, the
presenter facilitated an in-depth discussion on the poetic insights. While they were given
complete freedom in choosing and developing their visual imagery, they were provided
with an instructional rubric for developing the presentation (Figure 1). The rubric offered
clear guidelines, which served as a tool to contribute to the presentation’s quality in
addition to allaying anxiety about what to include.
Poetry Presentation Rubric
Figure 1. Sample rubric.
Questions Check Yes or No
Is the visual original? ___Yes ___No
Did the visual connect to theme of presentation? ___Yes ___No
Was the visual prominently displayed? ___Yes ___No
Was the theme of poem stated? ___Yes ___No
Was the poem paraphrased? ___Yes ___No
Were figures of speech used? ___Yes ___No
Was a central, controlling image identified? ___Yes ___No
Were lines quoted from the poem? ___Yes ___No
Did the audience ask questions or comment at end of
the presentation?
___Yes ___No
Did the student make eye contact? ___Yes ___No
Instructor’s Comments: Grade
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
72
The presentations engaged multiple learning styles—visual, auditory, kinesthetic,
and tactile, as students learned from each other. Some students were bewildered initially,
especially those who labeled themselves uncreative. Humans often approach new
challenges “with fear rather than mystery and wonder” (Costa & Kallick, 2004, p. 31). To
alleviate the fear, this creative assignment gave students academic freedom to explore
artistic alternatives. Inevitably, intelligent feedback and rich discourse ensued among
peers, even timid ones, making the assignment inspiring and meaningful. This assignment
generated enthusiasm while simultaneously challenging students to step outside the box.
Infusing visuals in a poetry assignment did more than help students meet learning
objectives. The creation and presentation of an original visual analysis also helped
students become more adept self-directed learners:
• Making choices while working independently and interdependently throughout
the process;
• Adding depth to what was learned from multiple interpretations;
• Building confidence;
• Deepening engagement;
• Connecting with the human factor in course material;
• Improving verbal communication skills;
• Learning from peers who shared their efforts, insights, and creations;
• Sparking new understandings of the poetry; and
• Employing their strengths and hidden talents.
This assignment also helped students realize their capacity to be original critical
thinkers. Reflecting on their learning experiences, many students reported feeling initially
overwhelmed by the requirements of this assignment. Once fear abated, they discovered
that multiple readings of the poem provided clarity. Most students felt the greatest
challenge was to create a visual that complemented their understanding of the poem. They
discovered that literal clarity sparked creativity and confidence; thus, pride in their artistic
work helped alleviate the fear of public speaking. Like most students, they anxiously
awaited feedback. Unlike class assignments that were assessed using a question-answer
format, this assignment introduced an additional way to discover what students know and
how they think; therefore, in assessing this assignment, knowledge acquisition was not as
important as knowledge production. Students produced a product and were rewarded.
The Honors Council objectives, delineated earlier, were designed to help students
not only with their oral and written communication skills but also to help students
embrace their independence while becoming better critical thinkers and to become skilled
at recognizing and understanding the methods scholars use to think about problems,
formulate theories, conduct research, and reach conclusions. This assignment, infusing
visuals in poetry critiques, encompassed aspects of all five objectives. Most remarkable
was that one work of art inspired the creation of another work of art.
Discussion
The methods of learning and assessment discussed are only a few samples of the
use of innovative learning activities and nontraditional approaches to assessment that can
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
73
foster student reflection, self-direction in learning, and potential transformation, the
beginning of a potentially productive investigation of SDL and its connection to Honors
courses and Honors students.
In the classes discussed, the approaches to course material were shifted from
instructor to student. Rather than listening to a professor’s humdrum lecture or witnessing
a circuitous discussion, students undertook novel approaches that created excitement and
participation. Evoked excitement stemmed from truly personal student involvement:
writing journals, acting out rewritten Canterbury Tales, and presenting visual poetic
interpretative critiques. An added bonus to the enhanced academic experiences was the
incorporation of creative abilities, allowing both professor and students to celebrate
individual talents. These approaches are not limited to these singular course experiences.
They are available, with appropriate specific curriculum restructuring, to any instructor
with the determination to encourage SDL in the classroom.
In an attempt to further promote self-directed learning and build student
confidence in those classes already discussed, students could be engaged in the actual
construction of assessment rubrics. This hands-on experience would give students the
opportunity to create their own assessment instrument. For example, in the imagitive
approach, after the instructor provides the students with a thorough explanation of the
project, including its basis and rationale, they could be asked to produce their own rubric.
Andrade (2000) notes that rubrics are valuable because they support the development of
sophisticated thinking skills. Student development of the rubric could enhance the impact
on thinking skills and add the dimension of promoting SDL. Each student could write
five items. Taking their lists, students could form groups of three and be asked to
consolidate them into one list of eight items. After consolidation is complete, the class as a
whole could post the lists and then vote on ten items that will comprise the grading rubric
to assess the poetry assignment. Not only will the students have more ownership in the
project but, in addition, it will be interesting to note the variance in a student-generated
evaluation instrument compared with that of the professor’s rubric. Will the students
emphasize the creative, abstract aspects more?
Another area that invites further research includes SDL indicator studies. Is
involvement in assignments and assessments such as those described in this article
associated with measurable increases in readiness for self-directed learning? In the classes
described, the SDL indicator instrument (Guglielmino, 1978) could be given at the
beginning and end of the courses to see if there is any change in how the students perceive
themselves. Along these same lines, giving the 4MAT (McCarthy & McCarthy, 2006) or
some similar instrument in conjunction with the SDL indicator might provide useful
insights into whether or not there is an identifiable connection between learning styles and
SDL in this population of Honors students. If there appears to be a relationship among
these factors, the studies could be widened to include other campuses, other professors,
and other Honors courses.
Although most instructors come to Honors teaching without specific training, that
should not limit them in providing an Honors education for students. Reviewing various
techniques that promote self-directed learning and incorporating those techniques into
Honors classrooms should be an integral part of instructional process. Implementation of
those techniques into various methods of evaluation and assessment promotes selfFostering
SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
74
directed learning in students who do not exhibit it, and further develops self-directed
learning attributes in those who already embrace it.
References
Andrade, H. G. (2000). Using rubrics to promote thinking and learning. Educational
Leadership, 57(5). Retrieved from http://74.125.155.132/scholar?q=cache:
TcJk38hU6k4J:scholar.google.com/+using+rubrics+to+promote+thinking+and+le
arning&hl=en&as_sdt=0,10
Boud, D. (2001). Using journal writing to enhance reflective practice. New Directions for
Adult & Continuing Education, (90), 9-18. doi:10.1002/ace.16
Brockman, J. (2001). A somatic epistemology for education. The Educational Forum,
65(4), 328-334.
Costa, A. L., & Kallick, B. (2004). Assessment strategies for self-directed learning.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Dirkx, J. (2001). The power of feelings: Emotion, imagination, and the construction of
meaning in adult learning. New Directions for Adult & Continuing Education,
(89), 63-72. doi: 10.1002/ace.9
Dominice, P. (2000). Learning from our lives: Using educational biographies with adults.
San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
English, L., & Gillen, M. (2001). Journal writing in practice: From vision to reality. New
Directions for Adult & Continuing Education, (90), 87-94. doi:10.1002/ace.9
Foehr, R. P., & Schiller, S. A. (1997). The spiritual side of writing. Portsmouth, NH:
Boynton/Cook.
Garrison, D. R. (1992). Critical thinking and self-directed learning in adult education: An
analysis of responsibility and control issues. Adult Education Quarterly, 42, 136-
148. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/custom/portlets/
recordDetails/detailmini.jsp?_nfpb=true&_&ERICExtSearch_SearchValue_0=EJ4
42483&ERICExtSearch_SearchType_0=no&accno=EJ442483
Gibbons, M. (2008). Motivating students and teaching them to motivate themselves.
Retrieved from http://www.selfdirectedlearning.com/article2.html
Guglielmino, L. M. (1978). Development of the Self-Directed Learning Readiness Scale.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 38, (11A), p. 6467A (University Microfilms
No. AAC78-06004).
Hiemstra, R. (2001). Uses and benefits of journal writing. New Directions for Adult &
Continuing Education, (90), 19-26. doi:10.1002/ace.17
Jarvis, P. (2001). Journal writing in higher education. New Directions for Adult &
Continuing Education, (90), 79-86. doi:10.1002/ace.23
Karpiak, I. (2002). Writing our life: Adult learning and teaching through autobiography.
Canadian Journal of University Continuing Education, 26(1), 31-50.
Karpiak, I. (2005). More than artistry: The integral aspect of autobiography. Canadian
Journal of University Continuing Education, 31(1), 87-107.
Kerka, S. (2002a). Journal writing as an adult learning tool. (Practice application brief
no. 22). Columbus, OH: ERIC Clearinghouse on Adult, Career, and Vocational
Education. Retrieved from http://www.ericacve.org/pubs.asp
Fostering SDL in the Honors Classroom
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
75
Kerka, S. (2002b). Somatic/Embodied learning and adult education. (Trends and issues
alert no. 32). Columbus, OH: ERIC Clearinghouse on Adult, Career, and
Vocational Education. Retrieved from http://www.ericacve.org/pubs.asp
Lawrence, R. (2005). Knowledge construction as contested terrain: Adult learning through
artistic expression. New Directions for Adult & Continuing Education, (107), 3-11.
doi: 10.1002/ace.184
Little, D., & Felten, P. (2010, February). Reflections on helping students learn: Thriving
in academe. NEA Higher Education Advocate, 27(3), 5-8.
McCarthy, B., & McCarthy, D. (2006). Teaching around the 4MAT cycle: Designing
instruction for diverse learners with diverse learning styles. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Corwin Press.
Merriam, S. B., Caffarella, R. S., & Baumgartner, L. M. (2007). Learning in adulthood
(3rd ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Mezirow, J., & Associates. (1990). Fostering critical reflection in adulthood: A guide to
transformative and emancipatory learning. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Mezirow, J., & Associates. (2000). Learning as transformation: Critical perspectives on a
theory in progress. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Mok, M. (2010). Self-directed learning oriented assessments: Assessment that informs
learning & empowers the learner. Mississauga, Ontario: Pace.
National Collegiate Honors Council. (2008). Designing an honors course. Retrieved from
http://www.nchchonors.org/honorscoursedesign.shtml
Stokes, S. (2007). Visual literacy in teaching and learning: A literature perspective.
Electronic Journal for the Integration of Technology in Education, 1(1). Retrieved
from http://ejite.isu.edu/Volume1No1/Stokes.html
Tisdell, E. J. (2003). Exploring spirituality and culture in adult and higher education. San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Nancy D. McDonald (mcdonaln@palmbeachstate.edu) is Associate Professor of English
and Honors Liaison at Palm Beach State College in Florida. She is a doctoral student in
Educational Leadership at Florida Atlantic University. In addition to her teaching
assignments, she served as a human rights observer in Haiti during the 1990s.
Idell McLaughlin (mclaughi@palmbeachstate.edu) is Associate Professor and Department
Chair of English at Palm Beach State College in Florida. In addition, she serves as
advisor for the Alpha Gamma Sigma Chapter of Phi Theta Kappa. She is also a doctoral
student in Educational Leadership at Florida Atlantic University.
International Journal of Self-Directed Learning Volume 7, Number 2, Fall 2010
76

The post FOSTERING SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING IN AN HONORS CLASSROOM THROUGH UNCONVENTIONAL METHODS AND ASSESSMENT Nancy D. McDonald and Idell McLaughlin presented by learnlikeaboss.com appeared first on LearnLikeABoss - Custom Mobile Training Apps Using The Power of Gamification.


Viewing all articles
Browse latest Browse all 41

Latest Images

Trending Articles





Latest Images